[go: up one dir, main page]

Jump to content

Joke

From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia

This is an old revision of this page, as edited by Smithriedel (talk | contribs) at 17:59, 20 August 2015 (Psychology). The present address (URL) is a permanent link to this revision, which may differ significantly from the current revision.

A joke uses words within a specific and well-defined narrative structure to make people laugh. It takes the form of a story, usually with dialogue, and ends in a punchline, often the final sentence. The punchline is the point at which the teller shows the audience the unexpected alternate meaning of the story, generating the humor in the joke. This article discusses jokes as folklore, the history of jokes, kinds of jokes, jokes as literature, and the psychology of joke telling (and listening).

A good joke is succinct, containing no more detail than is needed to set the scene for the punchline at the end. In the case of riddle jokes or one-liners the setting is implicitly understood, leaving only the dialogue and punchline to be verbalized. It is in the punchline that the audience becomes aware that the story contains a second, conflicting meaning. This can be done using a pun or other word play, a logical incompatibility, nonsense or other means. The listener is abruptly forced to switch from their original understanding of the text to an alternate, frequently more problematic, meaning.[1] This realization (hopefully, but not always) elicits laughter in the listener. A noted linguist, Robert Hetzron, offers this definition:

"A joke is a short humorous piece of oral literature in which the funniness culminates in the final sentence, called the punchline… In fact, the main condition is that the tension should reach its highest level at the very end. No continuation relieving the tension should be added. As for its being "oral," it is true that jokes may appear printed, but when further transferred, there is no obligation to reproduce the text verbatim, as in the case of poetry.[2]

Identified as one of the simple forms of oral literature by the Dutch linguist André Jolles,[3] jokes are passed along anonymously. They are told in both private and public settings; a single person tells a joke to his friend in the natural flow of conversation, or a set of jokes is told to a group as part of scripted entertainment. Jokes are also passed along in written form or—more recently—through electronic messaging systems. Internet joking has indeed become a major method of transmission. Either as written narratives or graphic cartoons, jokes are sent through email to friends and acquaintances; individuals joking with each other in a physical space have been replaced here by electronic social groups. This correlates with the new understanding of the internet as an "active folkloric space" with evolving social and cultural forces and clearly identifiable performers and audiences.[4] Along with individual transmission of jokes to email contacts, internet services are also available to provide a fresh joke-a-day to your email inbox or archive joke collections on electronic bulletin boards.

Jokes are a form of humor, but not all humor is a joke. Some humorous forms which are not jokes are: involuntary humor, situational humor, practical jokes, stand-up comedy, anecdotes, Charlie Chaplin. All of these are humorous, but none of them is a verbal joke. The Shaggy dog story is in a class of its own as an anti-joke; although presenting as a joke, it contains a long drawn-out narrative of time, place and character, rambles through many pointless inclusions and finally fails to deliver a punchline. Also, humor which is generated through performance can be funny but is not considered a joke. For the joke by definition contains the humor in the words (usually the punchline), not in the delivery. Stand-up comics, comedians and slapstick work with comic timing, precision and rhythm in their performance, relying as much on actions as on the verbal punchline to evoke laughter. This distinction has been formulated in the popular saying "A comic says funny things; a comedian says things funny".[note 1] Here we will only concern ourselves with verbal jokes, and leave performance comedy aside.

Telling Jokes

Telling a joke is a cooperative effort;[5][6] it requires that the teller and the audience mutually agree in one form or another to understand the narrative which follows as a joke. In a study of Conversation analysis, the sociologist Harvey Sacks describes in detail the sequential organization in the telling a single joke. "This telling is composed, as for stories, of three serially ordered and adjacently placed types of sequences … the preface [framing], the telling, and the response sequences."[7] Folklorists expand this to include the context of the joking. Who is telling what jokes to whom? And why is he telling them when?[8][9] The context of the joke telling in turn leads into a study of joking relationships, a term coined by anthropologists to refer to social groups within a culture who engage in institutionalized banter and joking.

Framing: Have you heard the one…

Framing is done with a (frequently formulaic) expression which keys the audience in to expect a joke. "Have you heard the one…", "Reminds me of a joke I heard…", "So, a lawyer and a doctor…"; these conversational markers are just a few examples of linguistic frames used to start a joke. Regardless of the frame used, it creates a social space and clear boundaries around the narrative which follows.[10] Audience response to this initial frame can be acknowledgement and anticipation of the joke to follow. It can also be a dismissal, as in "this is no joking matter" or "this is no time for jokes".

Within its performance frame, joke-telling is labeled as a culturally marked form of communication. Both the performer and audience understand it to be set apart from the "real" world. "An elephant walks into a bar…"; a native English speaker automatically understands that this is the start of a joke, and the story that follows is not meant to be taken at face value (i.e. it is non-bona-fide communication).[11] The framing itself invokes a play mode; if the audience is unable or unwilling to move into play, then nothing will seem funny.[12]

Telling

Following its linguistic framing the joke, in the form of a story, can be told. It is not required to be verbatim text like other forms of oral literature such as riddles and proverbs. The teller can and does modify the text of the joke, depending both on memory and the present audience. The important characteristic is that the narrative is succinct, containing only those details which lead directly to an understanding and decoding of the punchline. This requires that it support the same (or similar) divergent scripts which are to be embodied in the punchline.[13]

The narrative always contains a protagonist who becomes the "butt" or target of the joke. This labeling serves to develop and solidify stereotypes within the culture. It also enables researchers to group and analyze the creation, persistence and interpretation of joke cycles around a certain character. Some people are naturally better performers than others, however anyone can tell a joke because the comic trigger is contained in the narrative text and punchline. A joke poorly told is still funny unless the punchline gets mangled.

Punchline

The punchline is intended to make the audience laugh. A linguistic interpretation of this punchline / response is elucidated by Victor Raskin in his Script-based Semantic Theory of Humor. Humor is evoked when a trigger, contained in the punchline, causes the audience to abruptly shift its understanding of the story from the primary (or more obvious) interpretation to a secondary, opposing interpretation. "The punchline is the pivot on which the joke text turns as it signals the shift between the [semantic] scripts necessary to interpret [re-interpret] the joke text."[14] To produce the humor in the verbal joke, the two interpretations (i.e. scripts) need to be both compatible with the joke text AND opposite or incompatible with each other.[15] Thomas R. Shultz, a psychologist, independently expands Raskin’s linguistic theory to include "two stages of incongruity: perception and resolution." He explains that "… incongruity alone is insufficient to account for the structure of humour. […] Within this framework, humour appreciation is conceptualized as a biphasic sequence involving first the discovery of incongruity followed by a resolution of the incongruity."[16][17] Resolution generates laughter.

This is the point at which the field of neurolinguistics offers some insight into the cognitive processing involved in this abrupt laughter at the punchline. Studies by the cognitive science researchers Coulson and Kutas directly address the theory of script switching articulated by Raskin in their work.[18] The article "Getting it: Human event-related brain response to jokes in good and poor comprehenders" measures brain activity in response to reading jokes.[19] Additional studies by others in the field support more generally the theory of two-stage processing of humor, as evidenced in the longer processing time they require.[20] In the related field of neuroscience, it has been shown that the expression of laughter is caused by two partially independent neuronal pathways: an ‘involuntary’ or ‘emotionally driven’ system and a ‘voluntary’ system.[21] This study adds credence to the common experience when exposed to an off-color joke; a laugh is followed in the next breath by a disclaimer: "Oh, that’s bad…" Here the multiple steps in cognition are clearly evident in the stepped response, the perception being processed just a breath faster than the resolution of the moral / ethical content in the joke.

Responding

Expected response to a joke is laughter. The joke teller hopes the audience "gets it" and is entertained. This leads to the premise that a joke is actually an "understanding test" between individuals and groups.[22] If you don’t get the joke, you are not understanding the two scripts which are contained in the narrative as they were intended. Or you do "get it" and don’t laugh; it might be too obscene, too gross or too dumb for the current audience. A woman might respond differently to a joke told by a male colleague around the water cooler than she would to the same joke overheard in the Ladies’ Room. A joke about butt cracks is funnier told on the playground at elementary school then on a college campus. The same joke will elicit different responses in different settings. The punchline in the joke remains the same, however it is more or less appropriate depending on the current context.

Shifting Contexts, Shifting Texts

This takes us back to our original questions: Who is telling the joke to whom, and why are they telling it now? It would not be repeated if it did not mean something.The context explores the specific social situation in which joking occurs.[23] The narrator automatically modifies the text of the joke to be acceptable to different audiences, while at the same time supporting the same divergent scripts in the punchline. The vocabulary used in telling the same joke at a frat party and to your grandmother might well vary. In each situation it is important to identify both the narrator and the audience as well as their relationship with each other. This varies to reflect the complexities of a matrix of different social factors: age, sex, race, ethnicity, kinship, political views, religion, power relationship, etc. When you consider the potential combinations of such factors between the narrator and the audience, then a single joke can take on infinite shades of meaning for each unique social setting.

The context, however, should not be confused with the function of the joking. "Function is essentially an abstraction made on the basis of a number of contexts."[24] In one long-term observation of men coming off the late shift at a local café, joking with the waitresses was used to ascertain sexual availability for the evening. Different types of jokes, going from general to topical into explicitly sexual humor signaled openness on the part of the waitress for a connection.[25][26] This study describes how jokes and joking are used to communicate much more than just good humor. That is a single example of the function of joking in a social setting, but there are others. Sometimes jokes are used simply to get to know someone better. What makes them laugh, what do they find funny? Jokes concerning politics, religion or sexual topics can be used effectively to gage the attitude of the audience to any one of these topics. They can also be used as a marker of group identity, signaling either inclusion or exclusion for the group. Among pre-adolescents, "dirty" jokes allow them to share information about their changing bodies.[27] And sometimes joking is just simple entertainment for a group of friends.

Joking Relationships

The context of joking in turn leads into a study of joking relationships, a term coined by anthropologists to refer to social groups within a culture who take part in institutionalized banter and joking. These relationships can be either one-way or a mutual back and forth between partners. "The joking relationship is defined as a peculiar combination of friendliness and antagonism. The behaviour is such that in any other social context it would express and arouse hostility; but it is not meant seriously and must not be taken seriously. There is a pretence of hostility along with a real friendliness. To put it in another way, the relationship is one of permitted disrespect."[28] Joking relationships were first described by anthropologists within kinship groups in Africa. But they have since been identified in cultures around the world, where jokes and joking are used to mark and re-inforce appropriate boundaries of a relationship.[29]

Joking relationships do not always remain friendly and harmless. The practice of brutalized hazing as an initiation rite into some social groups grows out of such relationships of "permitted disrespect".

Printed jokes and the solitary laugh

There are many joke books in print today; a search on the internet provides a plethora of titles available for purchase. They can be read alone for solitary entertainment, or used to stock up on new jokes to entertain friends. Some people try to find a deeper meaning in jokes, for example "Plato and a Platypus Walk into a Bar... Understanding Philosophy Through Jokes."[30][note 2] However a deeper meaning is not necessary to appreciate their inherent entertainment value.[31] Magazines frequently use jokes and cartoons as filler for the printed page. Reader’s Digest closes out many articles with an (unrelated) joke at the bottom of article. The New Yorker was first published in 1925 with the stated goal of being a "sophisticated humor magazine" and is still known for its cartoons.

The practice of printers to use jokes and cartoons as page fillers was also widely used in the broadsides and chapbooks of the 19th century and earlier. With the increase in literacy in the general population and the growth of the printing industry, these publications were the most common forms of printed material between the 16th and 19th centuries throughout Europe and North America. Along with reports of events, executions, ballads and verse they also contained jokes. Only one of many broadsides archived in the Harvard library is described as "1706. Grinning made easy; or, Funny Dick’s unrivalled collection of curious, comical, odd, droll, humourous, witty, whimsical, laughable, and eccentric jests, jokes, bulls, epigrams, &c. With many other descriptions of wit and humour."[32] These cheap publications, ephemera intended for mass distribution, were read alone, read aloud, posted and discarded.

Earlier during the 15th century[33] the printing revolution spread across Europe following the development of the movable type printing press. This was coupled with the growth of literacy in all social classes. Printers turned out Jestbooks along with Bibles to meet both lowbrow and highbrow interests of the populace. One early anthology of jokes was the Facetiae by the Italian Poggio Bracciolini, first published in 1470. The popularity of this jest book can be measured on the twenty editions of the book documented alone for the 15th century. Another popular form was a collection of jests, jokes and funny situations attributed to a single character in a more connected, narrative form of the picaresque novel. Examples of this are the characters of Rabelais in France, Till Eulenspiegel in Germany, Lazarillo de Tormes in Spain and Master Skelton in England. There is also a jest book ascribed to William Shakespeare, the contents of which appear to both inform and borrow from his plays. All of these early jestbooks corroborate both the rise in the literacy of the European populations and the general quest for leisure activities during the Renaissance in Europe.[34]

The earliest extant joke book predates the printing press by a millennium; it is from the 4th century A.D. The ‘‘Philogelos’‘ (‘‘The Laughter Lover’‘) is (hand-)written in Greek and contains a collection of 265 jokes by Hierocles and Philagrius. The humor in this collection is surprisingly familiar, even though the typical protagonists are less recognizable to contemporary readers: the absent-minded professor, the eunuch, and people with hernias or bad breath. The ‘‘Philogelos’‘ even contains a joke similar to Monty Python's "Dead Parrot" sketch.[35]

One of the world’s oldest jokes was included in a political treatise inscribed on a roll of papyrus: "How do you entertain a bored pharaoh? You sail a boatload of young women dressed only in fishing nets down the Nile and urge the pharaoh to go catch a fish." Funny or not, this Egyptian riddle joke has been dated at 1600 B.C.[36] Looking further, all "oldest" jokes identified have 2 things in common: firstly, they were all written down, and secondly, their structure is remarkably similar to modern day jokes.[note 3]

Any joke documented from the past has been saved through happenstance rather than design. Jokes do not belong to refined culture, but rather to the entertainment and leisure of all classes. As such, any printed versions were considered ephemera, i.e., temporary documents created for a specific purpose and intended to be thrown away. Many of these early jokes deal with scatological and sexual topics, entertaining to all social classes but not to be valued and saved.

Electronic Joking

The advent of electronic communications at the end of the 20th century has broken the mold. Jokes are no longer oral but passed along as text or graphics; they are not told in at a social gathering but forwarded to email contacts. Folklorists and other social scientists are forced to leave the physical world to venture out into the WWW, where new traditions evolve bereft of observable response and context. A verbal joke or cartoon is emailed to a friend or posted on a bulletin board; the only response is a replied email with a :-) or LOL, or a forward on to further recipients. Interaction is limited to the computer screen and for the most part solitary. While preserving the text of a joke, both context and variants are lost in internet joking; for the most part emailed jokes are passed along verbatim.[37] The framing of the joke frequently occurs in the subject line: "RE: laugh for the day" or something similar. The forward of an email joke can increase the number of recipients exponentially; 5 x forwarding a joke to 5 recipients = 3,125 recipients in the course of an afternoon.

Internet joking forces a re-evaluation of social spaces and social groups. They are no longer only defined by physical presence and locality, they also exist in the connectivity in cyberspace.[38] "The computer networks appear to make possible communities that, although physically dispersed, display attributes of the direct, unconstrained, unofficial exchanges folklorists typically concern themselves with."[39] This is particularly evident in the spread of topical jokes, "that genre of lore in which whole crops of jokes spring up seemingly overnight around some sensational event … flourish briefly and then disappear, as the mass media move on to fresh maimings and new collective tragedies.’’[40]

Dissemination of the 9/11 joke cycle

An interesting study by the folklorist Bill Ellis documents for the first time how an evolving cycle was circulated over the internet.[41] By accessing message boards that specialized in humor immediately following the 9/11 disaster, Ellis was able to observe in real time both the topical jokes being posted electronically and responses to the jokes. "Previous folklore research has been limited to collecting and documenting successful jokes, and only after they had emerged and come to folklorists' attention. Now, an Internet-enhanced collection allows us a time machine, as it were, where we can observe what happens in the period before the risible moment, when attempts at humor are unsuccessful."[42] Access to archived message boards also enables us to track the development of a single joke thread in the context of a more complicated virtual conversation.[43] Using the tools of this new technology, it is possible to study the evolving discourse of a joke cycle as a response to a national or world disaster.

Joke Cycles

A joke cycle is a collection of jokes about a single target or situation which displays consistent narrative structure and type of humor. Some well-known cycles are elephant jokes using nonsense humor, dead baby jokes incorporating black humor and light bulb jokes, which describe all kinds of operational stupidity. Joke cycles can center on ethnic groups, professions (viola jokes), catastrophes, settings (…walks into a bar), absurd characters (wind-up dolls) or logical mechanisms which generate the humor (Knock-knock jokes). A joke can be reused in different joke cycles; an example of this is the same Head & Shoulders joke refitted to the tragedies of Vic Morrow, Admiral Mountbatten and the crew of the Challenger space shuttle.[note 4][44] These cycles seem to appear spontaneously, spread rapidly across countries and borders only to dissipate after some time.

Folklorists and others have studied individual joke cycles in an attempt to understand their function and significance within the culture. Below is a partial list of joke cycles circulated in the recent past, footnoted with monographs analyzing and interpreting their individual meanings. See also other joke cycles listed in Wikipedia for a more extensive listing.

3

Tragedies and catastrophes

As with the 9/11 disaster discussed above, cycles attach themselves to celebrities or national catastrophes such as Princess Diana, Michael Jackson or the Space Shuttle Challenger disaster. These cycles arise regularly as a response to terrible unexpected events which command the national news. An in-depth analysis of the Challenger joke cycle documents a change in the type of humor circulated following the disaster, from February to March 1986. "It shows that the jokes appeared in distinct ‘waves’, the first responding to the disaster with clever wordplay and the second playing with grim and troubling images associated with the event…The primary social function of disaster jokes appears to be to provide closure to an event that provoked communal grieving, by signaling that it was time to move on and pay attention to more immediate concerns."[66]

Ethnic jokes

The sociologist Christie Davies has written extensively on ethnic jokes told in countries around the world.[67] In ethnic jokes he finds that the ‘stupid’ ethnic target in the joke is no stranger to the culture, but rather a peripheral social group (geographic, economic, cultural, linguistic) well known to the joke tellers.[68] So Americans tell jokes about Polacks and Italians, Germans tell jokes about Ostfriesens, and the English tell jokes about the Irish. In a review of Davies’ theories it is said that "For Davies, [ethnic] jokes are more about how joke tellers imagine themselves than about how they imagine those others who serve as their putative targets…The jokes thus serve to center one in the world – to remind people of their place and to reassure them that they are in it."[69]

Absurdities and Gallows Humor

A third category of joke cycles identifies absurd characters as the butt: for example the grape, the dead baby or the elephant. Beginning in the 1960s, social and cultural interpretations of these joke cycles, spearheaded by the folklorist Alan Dundes, began to appear in academic journals. Dead baby jokes are posited to reflect societal changes and guilt caused by widespread use of contraception and abortion beginning in the 1960s.[note 5][70] Elephant jokes have been interpreted variously as stand-ins for American blacks during the Civil Rights Era[71] or as an "image of something large and wild abroad in the land captur[ing] the sense of counterculture" of the sixties.[72] These interpretations strive for a cultural understanding of the themes of these jokes which go beyond the simple collection and documentation undertaken previously by folklorists and ethnologists.

Classification Systems

As folktales and other types of oral literature became collectibles throughout Europe in the 19th century (Brothers Grimm et al.), folklorists and anthropologists of the time needed a system to organize these items. The Aarne–Thompson classification system was first published in 1910 by Antti Aarne, and later expanded by Stith Thompson to become the most renowned classification system for European folktales and other types of oral literature. Its final section addresses Anecdotes and jokes, listing traditional humorous tales ordered by their protagonist; "This section of the Index is essentially a classification of the older European jests, or merry tales – humorous stories characterized by short, fairly simple plots. …"[73][74] Due to its focus on older tale types and obsolete actors (ex. numbskull), the Aarne-Thompson Index does not provide much help in identifying and classifying the modern joke.

A more granular classification system used widely by folklorists and cultural anthropologists is the Thompson Motif Index, which separates tales into their individual story elements. This system enables jokes to be classified according to individual motifs included in the narrative: actors, items and incidents. It does not provide a system to classify the text by more than one element at a time while at the same time making it theoretically possible to classify the same text under multiple motifs.[75]

The Thompson Motif Index has spawned further specialized motif indices, each of which focuses on a single aspect of one subset of jokes. A sampling of just a few of these specialized indices have been listed under other motif indices. Here one can select an index for medieval Spanish folk narratives,[76] another index for linguistic verbal jokes,[77] and a third one for sexual humor.[78] To assist the researcher with this increasingly confusing situation, there are also multiple bibliographies of indices[79] as well as a How-To guide on creating your own index.[80]

Several difficulties have been identified with these systems of identifying oral narratives according to either tale types or story elements.[81] A first major problem is their hierarchical organization; one element of the narrative is selected as the major element, while all other parts are arrayed subordinate to this. A second problem with these systems is that the listed motifs are not qualitatively equal; actors, items and incidents are all considered side-by-side.[82] And because incidents will always have at least one actor and usually have an item, most narratives can be ordered under multiple headings. This leads to confusion about both where to order an item and where to find it. A third significant problem is that the "excessive prudery" common in the middle of the 20th century means that obscene, sexual and scatological elements were regularly ignored in many of the indices.[83]

The folklorist Robert Georges has summed up the concerns with these existing classification systems : "…Yet what the multiplicity and variety of sets and subsets reveal is that folklore [jokes] not only takes many forms, but that it is also multifaceted, with purpose, use, structure, content, style, and function all being relevant and important. Any one or combination of these multiple and varied aspects of a folklore example [such as jokes] might emerge as dominant in a specific situation or for a particular inquiry."[84] It has proven difficult to organize all different elements of a joke into a multi-dimensional classification system which could be of real value in the study and evaluation of this (primarily oral) complex narrative form. The General Theory of Verbal Humor or GTVH, developed by the linguists Victor Raskin and Salvatore Attardo, attempts to do exactly this. This classification system was developed specifically for jokes and later expanded to include longer types of humorous narratives.[85] Six different aspects of the narrative, labeled Knowledge Resources or KRs, can be evaluated largely independently of each other, and then combined into a concatenated classification label. These six KRs of the joke structure include:

  1. Script Opposition (SO) references the script opposition included in Raskin’s SSTH. This includes, among others, themes such as real (unreal), actual (non-actual), normal (abnormal), possible (impossible).
  2. Logical Mechanism (LM) refers to the mechanism which connects the different scripts in the joke. These can range from a simple verbal technique like a pun to more complex LMs such as faulty logic or false analogies.
  3. Situation (SI) can include objects, activities, instruments, props needed to tell the story.
  4. Target (TA) identifies the actor(s) who become the "butt" of the joke. This labeling serves to develop and solidify stereotypes of ethnic groups, professions, etc.
  5. Narrative strategy (NS) addresses the narrative format of the joke, as either a simple narrative, a dialogue, or a riddle. It attempts to classify the different genres and subgenres of verbal humor. In a subsequent study Attardo expands the NS to include oral and printed humorous narratives of any length, not just jokes.[86]
  6. Language (LA) "…contains all the information necessary for the verbalization of a text. It is responsible for the exact wording …and for the placement of the functional elements."[87]

As development of the GTVH progressed, a hierarchy of the KRs was established to partially restrict the options for lower level KRs depending on the KRs defined above them. For example, a lightbulb joke (SI) will always be in the form of a riddle (NS). Outside of these restrictions, the KRs can create a multitude of combinations, enabling a researcher to select jokes for analysis which contain only one or two defined KRs. It also allows for an evaluation of the similarity or dissimilarity of jokes depending on the similarity of their labels. "The GTVH presents itself as a mechanism … of generating [or describing] an infinite number of jokes by combining the various values that each parameter can take. … Descriptively, to analyze a joke in the GTVH consists of listing the values of the 6 KRs (with the caveat that TA and LM may be empty)."[88] This classification system provides a functional multi-dimensional label for any joke, and indeed any verbal humor.

Joke (and Humor) Research

Many academic disciplines lay claim to the study of jokes (and other forms of humor) as within their purview. Fortunately there are enough jokes, good, bad and worse, to go around. Unfortunately the studies of jokes from each of the interested disciplines brings to mind the tale of the Blind men and an elephant where the observations, although accurate reflections of their own competent methodological inquiry, frequently fail to grasp the beast in its entirety. This attests to the joke as a traditional narrative form which is indeed complex, concise and complete in and of itself.[89] It requires a "multidisciplinary, interdisciplinary, and cross-disciplinary field of inquiry"[90] to truly appreciate these nuggets of cultural insight.[note 6][91]

Psychology

Sigmund Freud was one of the first modern scholars to recognize jokes as an important object of investigation.[92] In his 1905 study "Jokes and their Relation to the Unconscious"[93] Freud describes the social nature of humor and illustrates his text with many examples of contemporary Viennese jokes.[94] His work is particularly noteworthy in this context because Freud distinguishes in his writings between jokes, humor and the comic.[95] These are distinctions which become easily blurred in many subsequent studies where everything funny tends to be gathered under the umbrella term of "humor", making for a much more diffuse discussion.

Since the publication of Freud’s study, psychologists have continued to explore humor and jokes in their quest to explain, predict and control an individual’s "sense of humor". Why do people laugh? Why do people find something funny? Can jokes predict character, or vice versa, can character predict the jokes you laugh at? What is a "sense of humor"? A current review of the popular magazine "Psychology Today" lists over 200 articles discussing various aspects of humor; in psychospeak the subject area has become both an emotion to measure and a tool to use in diagnostics and treatment. A new psychological assessment tool, the Values in Action Inventory developed by the American psychologists Peterson and Seligman includes humor (and playfulness) as one of the core character strengths of an individual. As such, it could be a good predictor of life satisfaction.[96] For psychologists, it would be useful to measure both how much of this strength an individual has and how we can measurably increase it.

Done. A 2007 survey of existing tools to measure humor identified more than 60 psychological measurement instruments.[97] These measurement tools use many different approaches to quantify humor along with its related states and traits. There are tools to measure an individual’s physical response by their smile; the Facial Action Coding System (FACS) is one of several tools used to identify any one of multiple types of smiles.[98] Or the laugh can be measured to calculate the funniness response of an individual; multiple types of laughter have been identified. It must be stressed here that both smiles and laughter are not always a response to something funny. In trying to develop a measurement tool, most systems use "jokes and cartoons" as their test materials. However, because no two tools use the same jokes, and across languages this would not be feasible, how do you know the assessment objects are comparable? Moving on, whom do you ask to rate the sense of humor of an individual? Do you ask the person themselves, an impartial observer, or their family, friends and colleagues? Furthermore, has the current mood of the test subjects been considered; someone with a recent death in the family might not be much prone to laughter. Given the plethora of variants revealed by even a superficial glance at the problem,[99] it becomes evident that these paths of scientific inquiry are mined with problematic pitfalls and questionable solutions.

The psychologist Willibald Ruch has been very active in the research of humor. He has collaborated with the linguists Raskin and Attardo on their General Theory of Verbal Humor (GTVH) classification system. Their goal is to empirically test both the six autonomous classification types (KRs) and the hierarchical ordering of these KRs. Advancement in this direction would be a win-win for both fields of study; linguistics would have empirical verification of this multi-dimensional classification system for jokes, and psychology would have a standardized joke classification with which they could develop verifiably comparable measurement tools.

Linguistics

"The linguistics of humor has made gigantic strides forward in the last decade and a half and replaced the psychology of humor as the most advanced theoretical approach to the study of this important and universal human faculty."[100] This recent statement by one noted linguist and humor researcher describes, from his perspective, contemporary linguistic humor research. Linguists study words, how words are strung together to build sentences, how sentences create meaning which can be communicated from one individual to another, how our interaction with each other using words creates discourse. We have defined a joke as an oral narrative in which words and sentences are engineered to build toward a punchline. The linguist’s question is: what exactly makes the punchline funny? This question focuses on how the words used in the punchline create humor, in contrast to the psychologist’s concern (see above) with the audience response to the punchline. The assessment of humor by psychologists "is made from the individual’s perspective; e.g. the phenomenon associated with responding to or creating humor and not a description of humor itself."[101] Linguistics, on the other hand, endeavors to provide a precise description of what makes a text funny.[102]

Two major new linguistic theories have been developed and tested within the last decades. The first was advanced by Victor Raskin in "Semantic Mechanisms of Humor", published 1985.[103] While being a variant on the more general concepts of the Incongruity theory of humor, it is the first theory to identify its approach as exclusively linguistic. The Script-based Semantic Theory of Humor (SSTH) begins by identifying two linguistic conditions which make a text funny. It then goes on to identify the mechanisms involved in creating the punchline. This theory established the semantic/pragmatic foundation of humor as well as the humor competence of speakers.[note 7][104]

Several years later the SSTH was incorporated into a more expansive theory of jokes put forth by Raskin and his colleague Salvatore Attardo. In the General Theory of Verbal Humor, the SSTH was relabeled as a Logical Mechanism (LM) (referring to the mechanism which connects the different linguistic scripts in the joke) and added to 5 other independent Knowledge Resources (KR). Together these six KRs could now function as a multi-dimensional descriptive label for any piece of humorous text. This GTVH for jokes continues to be the only multi-dimensional classification system which even begins to address the complexity of this narrative form.

Linguistics has developed further methodological tools which can be applied to jokes: discourse analysis and conversation analysis of joking. Both of these subspecialties within the field focus on ‘naturally occurring’ language use, i.e. the analysis of real (usually recorded) conversations. One of these studies has already been discussed above, where Harvey Sacks describes in detail the sequential organization in the telling a single joke.[105] Discourse analysis emphasizes the entire context of social joking, the social interaction which cradles the words.

Folklore and Anthropology

Folklore and cultural anthropology have perhaps the strongest claims on jokes as belonging to their bailiwick. Jokes remain one of the few remaining forms of traditional folk literature transmitted orally in western cultures. Identified as one of the "simple forms" of oral literature by Andre Jolles[106] in 1930, they have been collected and studied since there were folklorists and anthropologists abroad in the lands. As a genre they were important enough at the beginning of the 20th century to be included under their own heading in the Aarne-Thompson index first published in 1910: Anecdotes and jokes.

Beginning in the 1960s, cultural researchers began to expand their role from collectors and archivists of "folk ideas"[107] to a more active role of interpreters of cultural artifacts. One of the foremost scholars active during this transitional time was the folklorist Alan Dundes. He started asking questions of tradition and transmission with the key observation that "No piece of folklore continues to be transmitted unless it means something, even if neither the speaker nor the audience can articulate what that meaning might be."[108] In the context of jokes, this then becomes the basis for further research. Why are we telling/hearing this joke right now? Only in this expanded perspective can we understand what it means to the participants.

This questioning resulted in a blossoming of monographs to explore the significance of many joke cycles. What is so funny about absurd nonsense elephant jokes? Why would we make light of dead babies? In an article on contemporary German jokes about Auschwitz and the Holocaust, Dundes justifies this research: "Whether one finds Auschwitz jokes funny or not is not an issue. This material exists and should be recorded. Jokes are always an important barometer of the attitudes of a group. The jokes exist and they obviously must fill some psychic need for those individuals who tell them and those who listen to them."[109] We also find a stimulating generation of new humor theories flourishing like mushrooms in the undergrowth: Elliott Oring’s theoretical discussions on "appropriate ambiguity" and Amy Carrell’s hypothesis of an "audience-based theory of verbal humor (1993)" to name just a few.

In his book "Humor and Laughter: an Anthropological Approach",[110] the anthropologist Mahadev Apte presents a solid case for his own academic perspective. "Two axioms underlie my discussion, namely, that humor is by and large culture based and that humor can be a major conceptual and methodological tool for gaining insights into cultural systems."[111] Apte goes on to call for legitimizing the field of humor research as "Humorology"; this would be a field of study incorporating an interdisciplinary character of humor studies.[112]

While the label "Humorology" has yet to become a household word, great strides are being made in the international recognition of this interdisciplinary field of research. The International Society for Humor Studies was founded in 1989 with the stated purpose to "promote, stimulate and encourage the interdisciplinary study of humor; to support and cooperate with local, national, and international organizations having similar purposes; to organize and arrange meetings; and to issue and encourage publications concerning the purpose of the society." It also publishes Humor: International Journal of Humor Research and holds yearly conferences to promote and inform its specialty

Computational Humor

Computational humor is a new field of study which uses computers to model humor;[113] it bridges the disciplines of computational linguistics and artificial intelligence. A primary ambition of this field is to develop computer programs which can both generate a joke and recognize a text snippet as a joke. Early programming attempts have dealt almost exclusively with punning because this lends itself to simple straightforward rules. These primitive programs display no intelligence; instead they work off a template with a finite set of pre-defined punning options upon which to build.

More sophisticated computer joke programs have yet to be developed. Based on our understanding of the SSTH / GTVH humor theories, it is easy to see why. The linguistic scripts (a.k.a. frames) referenced in in these theories include, for any given word, a "large chunk of semantic information surrounding the word and evoked by it [...] a cognitive structure internalized by the native speaker".[114] These scripts extend much further than the lexical definition of a word; they contain the speaker’s complete knowledge of the concept as it exists in his world. As insentient machines, computers lack the encyclopedic scripts which humans gain through life experience. They also lack the ability to gather the experiences needed to build wide-ranging semantic scripts and understand language in a broader context, a context that any child picks up in daily interaction with his environment.

Further development in this field must wait until computational linguists have succeeded in programming a computer with an ontological semantic natural language processing system. It is only "the most complex linguistic structures [which] can serve any formal and/or computational treatment of humor well"[115] Toy systems (i.e. dummy punning programs) are completely inadequate to the task. Despite the fact that the field of computational humor is small and underdeveloped, it is encouraging to note the many interdisciplinary efforts which are currently underway.[116] As this field grows in both understanding and methodology, it provides an ideal testbed for humor theories; you must firstly have cleanly defined rules in order to write a computer program around a theory.

Physiology of Laughter

In 1872 Charles Darwin published one of the first "comprehensive and in many ways remarkably accurate description of laughter in terms of respiration, vocalization, facial action and gesture and posture".[117] In this early study Darwin raises further questions about who laughs and why we laugh; the myriad responses since then illustrates the complexities of this behavior. In order to understand laughter in humans and other primates, the science of Gelotology (from the Greek gelos, meaning laughter) has been established; it is the study of laughter and its effects on the body from both a psychological and physiological perspective. While jokes can provoke laughter, laughter cannot be used as a one-to-one marker of jokes because there are multiple stimuli to laugher, humor being just one of them. (The other six causes of laughter listed are: social context, ignorance, anxiety, derision, acting apology, and tickling.)[118][119] As such, the study of laughter is a secondary albeit entertaining perspective in an understanding of jokes.

Notes

  1. ^ Generally attributed to Ed Wynn
  2. ^ NPR Interview with the authors Cathcart and Klein can be found at http://www.npr.org/templates/story/story.php?storyId=10158510
  3. ^ Shaped by modern expectations in translation? Hard to say.
  4. ^ How do we know that ___ had dandruff? They found his/her head and shoulders on the ___.
  5. ^ Contraceptive pills were first approved for use in the USA in 1960.
  6. ^ Our focus here is with the contemporary state of joke research. A more extensive survey of the history of various humor theories can be found under the topic Theories of humor.
  7. ^ i.e. The necessary and sufficient conditions for a text to be funny

Footnotes

  1. ^ (Raskin 1985, p. 99)
  2. ^ (Hetzron 1991, pp. 65–66)
  3. ^ (Jolles 1930)
  4. ^ (Dorst 1990, p. 183)
  5. ^ (Raskin 1985, p. 103)
  6. ^ (Attardo & Chabanne 1992)
  7. ^ (Sacks 1974, pp. 337–353)
  8. ^ (Dundes 1980, pp. 20–32)
  9. ^ (Bauman 1975)
  10. ^ (Sims & Stephens 2005, p. 141)
  11. ^ (Raskin 1992)
  12. ^ (Ellis 2002, p. 3), (Marcus 2001)
  13. ^ (Toelken 1996, p. 55)
  14. ^ (Carrell 2008, p. 308)
  15. ^ (Raskin 1985, p. 99)
  16. ^ (Shultz 1976, pp. 12–13)
  17. ^ (Carrell 2008, p. 312)
  18. ^ (Coulson & Kutas 1998)
  19. ^ (Coulson & Kutas 2001, pp. 71–74)
  20. ^ (Attardo 2008, pp. 125–126)
  21. ^ (Wild et al. 2003)
  22. ^ (Sacks 1974, p. 350)
  23. ^ (Dundes 1980, p. 23)
  24. ^ (Dundes 1980, pp. 23–24)
  25. ^ (Walle 1976)
  26. ^ (Oring 2008, p. 201)
  27. ^ (Sims & Stephens 2005, p. 39)
  28. ^ (Radcliffe-Brown 1940, p. 196)
  29. ^ (Apte 1985)
  30. ^ (Cathcart & Klein 2007)
  31. ^ (Berry 2013)
  32. ^ (Lane 1905)
  33. ^ (Ward & Waller 2000)
  34. ^ (Ward & Waller 2000)
  35. ^ (Adams 2008)
  36. ^ (Joseph 2008)
  37. ^ (Frank 2009, pp. 99–100)
  38. ^ (Mason 1998)
  39. ^ (Dorst 1990, pp. 180–181)
  40. ^ (Dorst 1990)
  41. ^ (Ellis 2002)
  42. ^ (Ellis 2002, p. 2)
  43. ^ (Ellis 2002)
  44. ^ (Gruner 1997, pp. 142–143)
  45. ^ (Smyth 1986)
  46. ^ (Oring 1987)
  47. ^ (Laszlo 1988)
  48. ^ (Dundes 1979)
  49. ^ (Davies 1998)
  50. ^ (Hirsch & Barrick 1980)
  51. ^ (Dundes 1971)
  52. ^ (Dundes 1985)
  53. ^ http://www.npr.org/blogs/npr-history-dept/2015/03/03/389865887/the-secret-history-of-knock-knock-jokes
  54. ^ (Dundes 1981)
  55. ^ (Kerman 1980)
  56. ^ (Davies 1999)
  57. ^ (Simons 1986)
  58. ^ (Smyth 1986)
  59. ^ (Oring 1987)
  60. ^ (Davies 2002)
  61. ^ (Kitchener 1991)
  62. ^ (Dundes & Pagter 1991)
  63. ^ (Dundes 1991)
  64. ^ (Rahkonen 2000)
  65. ^ (Hirsch 1964)
  66. ^ (Ellis 1991)
  67. ^ (Davies 1990)
  68. ^ (Davies 2008, pp. 163–165)
  69. ^ (Oring 2000)
  70. ^ (Dundes 1987, pp. 3–14)
  71. ^ (Dundes 1987, pp. 41–54)
  72. ^ (Oring 2008, p. 194)
  73. ^ (Brunvand 1968, p. 238)
  74. ^ (Dundes 1997)
  75. ^ (Dundes 1997)
  76. ^ (Goldberg 1998)
  77. ^ (Lew 1996)
  78. ^ (Legman 1968)
  79. ^ (Azzolina 1987)
  80. ^ (Jason 2000)
  81. ^ (Apo 1997)
  82. ^ (Dundes 1962)
  83. ^ (Dundes 1997, p. 198)
  84. ^ (Georges 1997, p. 111)
  85. ^ (Attardo 2001)
  86. ^ (Attardo 2001)
  87. ^ (Attardo 1994, p. 223)
  88. ^ (Attardo 2001, p. 27)
  89. ^ (Attardo & Chabanne 1992, p. 172)
  90. ^ (Apte 1988, p. 7) harv error: multiple targets (2×): CITEREFApte1988 (help)
  91. ^ (Dundes 1972)
  92. ^ (Carrell 2008, p. 304)
  93. ^ (Freud 1905)
  94. ^ (Oring 1984)
  95. ^ (Morreall 2008, p. 224)
  96. ^ (Ruch 2008, p. 47)
  97. ^ (Ruch 2008, p. 58)
  98. ^ (Furnham 2014)
  99. ^ (Ruch 2008, pp. 40–45)
  100. ^ (Raskin 1992, p. 91)
  101. ^ (Ruch 2008, p. 19)
  102. ^ (Ruch 2008, p. 25)
  103. ^ (Raskin 1985)
  104. ^ (Attardo 2001, p. 114)
  105. ^ (Sacks 1974)
  106. ^ (Jolles 1930)
  107. ^ (Dundes 1972)
  108. ^ (Dundes & Pagter 1987, p. vii)
  109. ^ (Dundes & Hauschild 1983, p. 250)
  110. ^ (Apte 1985)
  111. ^ (Apte 2002)
  112. ^ (Apte 1988) harv error: multiple targets (2×): CITEREFApte1988 (help)
  113. ^ (Mulder & Nijholt 2002)
  114. ^ (Raskin 1985, p. 46)
  115. ^ (Raskin 1996, p. 17/349)
  116. ^ (Hempelman 2008, p. 354)
  117. ^ (Ruch 2008, p. 24)
  118. ^ (Giles & Oxford 1970)
  119. ^ (Attardo 2008, pp. 116–117)

References

  • Adams, Stephen (2008). "Dead parrot sketch is 1600 years old: It's long been held that the old jokes are the best jokes - and Monty Python's Dead Parrot sketch is no different". The Telegraph. {{cite news}}: Invalid |ref=harv (help)
  • Apo, Satu (1997). "Motif". In Green, Thomas (ed.). Folklore An Encyclopedia of Beliefs, Customs, Tales, Music, and Art. Santa Barbara, CA: ABC-CLIO. pp. 563–564. {{cite book}}: Invalid |ref=harv (help)
  • Apte, Mahadev L. (1985). Humor and Laughter: an Anthropological Approach. Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press. {{cite book}}: Invalid |ref=harv (help)
  • Apte, Mahadev (1988). "Disciplinary boundaries in humorology: An anthropologist's ruminations". Humor: International Journal of Humor Research. 1 (1): 5–25. {{cite journal}}: Invalid |ref=harv (help)
  • Apte, Mahadev (1988). "Disciplinary boundaries in humorology: An anthropologist's ruminations". Humor: International Journal of Humor Research. 1 (1): 5–25. {{cite journal}}: Invalid |ref=harv (help)
  • Apte, Mahadev L. (2002). "Author Review of Humor and Laughter: an Anthropological Approach". Retrieved 10 August 2015. {{cite web}}: Invalid |ref=harv (help)
  • Attardo, Salvatore (1994). Linguistic Theories of Humor. Berlin, New York: Mouton de Gruyter. {{cite book}}: Invalid |ref=harv (help)
  • Attardo, Salvatore (2001). Humorous Texts: A Semantic and Pragmatic Analysis. Berlin: Walter de Gruyter. {{cite book}}: Invalid |ref=harv (help)
  • Attardo, Salvatore (2008). "A primer for the Linguistics of Humor". In Raskin, Victor (ed.). Primer of Humor Research: Humor Research 8. Berlin, New York: Mouton de Gruyter. pp. 101–156. {{cite book}}: Invalid |ref=harv (help)
  • Attardo, Salvatore; Chabanne, Jean-Charles (1992). "Jokes as a text type". HUMOR International Journal of Humor Research: Humor research east of the Atlantic. 5 (1/2): 165–176. {{cite journal}}: Invalid |ref=harv (help)
  • Azzolina, David (1987). Tale type- and motif-indices: An annotated bibliography. New York: Garland. {{cite book}}: Invalid |ref=harv (help)
  • Bauman, Richard (1975). "Verbal Art as Performance". American Anthropologist, New Series. 77 (2). Wiley: 290–311. Retrieved 2015-04-13. {{cite journal}}: Invalid |ref=harv (help)
  • Berry, William (2013). "The Joke's On Who?". Psychology Today (Feb 2013). {{cite journal}}: Invalid |ref=harv (help)
  • Bronner, Simon J., ed. (2007). The Meaning of folklore: the Analytical Essays of Alan Dundes. Logan, UT: Utah State University Press. {{cite book}}: Invalid |ref=harv (help)
  • Brunvand, Jan Harald (1968). The Study of American Folklore. New York, London: W.W. Norton. {{cite book}}: Invalid |ref=harv (help)
  • Carrell, Amy (2008). "Historical Views of Humor". In Raskin, Victor (ed.). Primer of Humor Research: Humor Research 8 (PDF). Berlin, New York: Mouton de Gruyter. pp. 303–332. {{cite book}}: Invalid |ref=harv (help)
  • Cathcart, Thomas; Klein, Daniel (2007). Plato and a Platypus Walk into a Bar... Understanding Philosophy Through Jokes. New York: Penguin Books. {{cite book}}: Invalid |ref=harv (help)
  • Coulson, Seana; Kutas, Marta (1998). "Frame-shifting and sentential integration". USCD Cognitive Science Technical Report. 4 (3–4). San Diego, CA: Technical Report CogSci.UCSD-98.03. {{cite journal}}: Invalid |ref=harv (help)
  • Coulson, Seana; Kutas, Marta (2001). "Getting it: Human event-related brain response to jokes in good and poor comprehenders". Neuroscience Letters 316 (316). {{cite journal}}: Invalid |ref=harv (help)
  • Davies, Christie (1990). Ethnic Humor Around the World: A comparative Analysis. Bloomington: Indiana University Press. {{cite book}}: Invalid |ref=harv (help)
  • Davies, Christie (1998). Jokes and Their Relation to Society. Walter de Gruyter. pp. 186–189. ISBN 3-11-016104-4. {{cite book}}: Invalid |ref=harv (help)
  • Davies, Christie (1999). "Jokes on the Death of Diana". In Walter, Julian Anthony; Walter, Tony (eds.). The Mourning for Diana. Berg Publishers. p. 255. ISBN 1-85973-238-0. {{cite book}}: Invalid |ref=harv (help)
  • Davies, Christie (2002). "Jokes about Newfies and Jokes told by Newfoundlanders". Mirth of Nations. Transaction Publishers. ISBN 0-7658-0096-9. {{cite book}}: Invalid |ref=harv (help)
  • Davies, Christie (2008). "Undertaking the Comparative Study of Humor". In Raskin, Victor (ed.). Primer of Humor Research: Humor Research 8. Berlin, New York: Mouton de Gruyter. pp. 157–182. {{cite book}}: Invalid |ref=harv (help)
  • Dorst, John (1990). "Tags and Burners, Cycles and Networks: Folklore in the Telectronic Age". Journal of Folklore Research. 27 (3). Bloomington and Indianapolis: Indiana University Press: 61–108. {{cite journal}}: Invalid |ref=harv (help)
  • Douglas, Mary (1975). "Jokes". In Mukerji, Chandra; Schudson, Michael (eds.). Rethinking Popular Culture: Contemporary Perspectives in Cultural Studies. Berkeley, CA: University of California. {{cite book}}: Invalid |ref=harv (help)
  • Dundes, Alan (1962). "From Etic to Emic Units in the Structural Study of Folktales". Journal of American Folklore. 75 (296): 95–105. {{cite journal}}: Invalid |ref=harv (help)
  • Dundes, Alan (1971). "A Study of Ethnic Slurs: The Jew and the Polack in the United States". Journal of American Folklore. 84 (332). The Journal of American Folklore, Vol. 84, No. 332: 186–203. doi:10.2307/538989. JSTOR 538989. {{cite journal}}: Invalid |ref=harv (help)
  • Dundes, Alan (1972). "Folk ideas as units of World View". In Bauman, Richard; Paredes, Americo (eds.). Toward New Perspectives in Folklore. Bloomington, IN: Trickster Press. pp. 120–134. {{cite book}}: Invalid |ref=harv (help)
  • Dundes, Alan (July 1979). "The Dead Baby Joke Cycle". Western Folklore. 38 (3). Western Folklore, Vol. 38, No. 3: 145–157. doi:10.2307/1499238. JSTOR 1499238. {{cite journal}}: Invalid |ref=harv (help)CS1 maint: date and year (link)
  • Dundes, Alan (1980). "Texture, text and context". Interpreting Folklore. Bloomington and Indianapolis: Indiana University Press. pp. 20–32. {{cite book}}: Invalid |ref=harv (help)
  • Dundes, Alan (1981). "Many Hands Make Light Work or Caught in the Act of Screwing in Light Bulbs". Western Folklore. 40 (3): 261–266. {{cite journal}}: Invalid |ref=harv (help)
  • Dundes, Alan (October–December 1985). "The J. A. P. and the J. A. M. in American Jokelore". The Journal of American Folklore. 98 (390). The Journal of American Folklore, Vol. 98, No. 390: 456–475. doi:10.2307/540367. JSTOR 540367. {{cite journal}}: Invalid |ref=harv (help)CS1 maint: date and year (link)
  • Dundes, Alan (1987). Cracking jokes: Studies of Sick Humor Cycles & Stereotypes. Berkley: Ten Speed Press. {{cite book}}: Invalid |ref=harv (help)
  • Dundes, Alan, ed. (1991). "Folk Humor". Mother Wit from the Laughing Barrel: Readings in the Interpretation of Afro-American Folklore. University Press of Mississippi. p. 612. ISBN 0-87805-478-2. {{cite book}}: Invalid |ref=harv (help)
  • Dundes, Alan (1997). "The Motif-Index and the Tale Type Index: A Critique". Journal of Folklore Research. 34 (3). Bloomington and Indianapolis: Indiana University Press: 195–202. {{cite journal}}: Invalid |ref=harv (help)
  • Dundes, Alan; Hauschild, Thomas (October 1983). "Auschwitz Jokes". Western Folklore. 42 (4). {{cite journal}}: Invalid |ref=harv (help)CS1 maint: date and year (link)
  • Dundes, Alan; Pagter, Carl R. (1987). When You're Up to Your Ass in Alligators: More Urban Folklore from the Paperwork Empire. Detroit: Wayne State University Press. {{cite book}}: Invalid |ref=harv (help)
  • Dundes, Alan; Pagter, Carl R. (1991). "The mobile SCUD Missile Launcher and other Persian Gulf Warlore: An American Folk Image of Saddam Hussein's Iraq". Western Folklore. 50: 303–322. {{cite journal}}: Invalid |ref=harv (help)
  • Ellis, Bill (1991). "The Last Thing ... Said: The Challenger Disaster Jokes and Closure". International Folklore Review (8). London: 110–124. {{cite journal}}: Invalid |ref=harv (help)
  • Ellis, Bill (2002). "Making a Big Apple Crumble". New Directions in Folklore (6). {{cite journal}}: Invalid |ref=harv (help)
  • Frank, Russel (2009). "The Forward as Folklore: Studying E-Mailed Humor". In Blank, Trevor J. (ed.). Folklore and the Internet. Logan, UT: Utah State University Press. pp. 98–122. {{cite book}}: Invalid |ref=harv (help)
  • Freedman, Matt; Hoffman, Paul (1980). How Many Zen Buddhists Does It Take to Screw In a Light Bulb?. New York. {{cite book}}: Invalid |ref=harv (help)CS1 maint: location missing publisher (link)
  • Freud, Sigmund (1905). Der Witz und seine Beziehung zum Unbewußten. Leipzig, Vienna. {{cite book}}: Invalid |ref=harv (help)CS1 maint: location missing publisher (link)
  • Furnham, Adrian (Oct 30, 2014). "The Surprising Psychology of Smiling: Natural or fake, each smile tells you something important about its wearer". {{cite journal}}: Cite journal requires |journal= (help); Invalid |ref=harv (help)CS1 maint: date and year (link)
  • Georges, Robert A. (1997). "The Centrality in Folkloristics of Motif and Tale Type". Journal of Folklore Research. 34 (3). Bloomington and Indianapolis: Indiana University Press. {{cite journal}}: Invalid |ref=harv (help)
  • Georges, Robert A.; Jones, Michael Owen (1995). Folkloristics : an Introduction. Bloomington and Indianapolis: Indiana University Press. {{cite book}}: Invalid |ref=harv (help)
  • Giles, H.; Oxford, G.S. (1970). "Towards a multidimensional theory of laughter causation and its social implications". Bulletin of British Psychology Society 23. 23: 97–105. {{cite journal}}: Invalid |ref=harv (help)
  • Goldberg, Harriet (1998). "Motif-Index of Medieval Spanish Folk Narratives". Medieval & Renaissance Texts & Studies. Tempe, AZ. {{cite book}}: Invalid |ref=harv (help)CS1 maint: location missing publisher (link)
  • Gruner, Charles R. (1997). The Game of Humor: A Comprehensive Theory of Why We Laugh. Piscataway, NJ: Transaction Publishers. ISBN 0-7658-0659-2. {{cite book}}: Invalid |ref=harv (help)
  • Hempelmann, Christian; Samson, Andrea C. (2008). "Cartoons: Drawn jokes?". In Raskin, Victor (ed.). Primer of Humor Research: Humor Research 8. Berlin, New York: Mouton de Gruyter. pp. 609–640. {{cite book}}: Invalid |ref=harv (help)
  • Hetzron, Robert (1991). "On the structure of punchlines". Humor: International Journal of Humor Research. 4 (1): 61–108. {{cite journal}}: Invalid |ref=harv (help)
  • Hirsch, K.; Barrick, M.E. (1980). "The Helen Keller Joke Cycle". Journal of American Folklore. 93 (370). The Journal of American Folklore, Vol. 93, No. 370: 441–448. doi:10.2307/539874. JSTOR 539874. {{cite journal}}: Invalid |ref=harv (help)
  • Hirsch, Robin (1964). "Wind-Up Dolls". Western Folklore. 23 (2). Western Folklore, Vol. 23, No. 2: 107–110. doi:10.2307/1498259. JSTOR 1498259. {{cite journal}}: Invalid |ref=harv (help)
  • Jason, Heda (2000). "Motif, type, and genre: a manual for compilation of indices & a bibliography of indices and indexing". FF Communications. 273. Helsinki: Suomalainen Tiedeakatemia. {{cite journal}}: Invalid |ref=harv (help)
  • Jolles, André (1930). Einfache Formen. Legende, Sage, Mythe, Rätsel, Spruch, Kasus, Memorabile, Märchen, Witz. Halle (Saale): Forschungsinstitut für Neuere Philologie Leipzig: Neugermanistische Abteilung; 2. {{cite book}}: Invalid |ref=harv (help)
  • Joseph, John (2008). "World's oldest joke traced back to 1900 BC". Reuters.
  • Kerman, Judith B. (1980). "The Light-Bulb Jokes: Americans Look at Social Action Processes". Journal of American Folklore. 93: 454–458. {{cite journal}}: Invalid |ref=harv (help)
  • Kitchener, Amy (1991). Explosive Jokes: A collection of Persian Gulf War Humor. Unpublished Manuscript. {{cite book}}: Invalid |ref=harv (help)
  • Lane, William Coolidge, ed. (1905). Catalogue of English and American chapbooks and broadside ballads in Harvard University Library. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University. {{cite book}}: Invalid |ref=harv (help)
  • Laszlo, Kurti (July–September 1988). "The Politics of Joking: Popular Response to Chernobyl". The Journal of American Folklore. 101 (401). The Journal of American Folklore, Vol. 101, No. 401: 324–334. doi:10.2307/540473. JSTOR 540473. {{cite journal}}: Invalid |ref=harv (help)
  • Legman, Gershon (1968). Rationale of the Dirty Joke: an Analysis of Sexual Humor. New York: Simon & Schuster. {{cite book}}: Invalid |ref=harv (help)
  • Lew, Robert (1996). An Ambiguity-based theory of the linguistic verbal joke in English (PDF). Poznań, Poland: unpublished thesis, Adam Mickiewicz University. {{cite book}}: Invalid |ref=harv (help)
  • Marcus, Adam (2001). "Laughter Shelved in Medicine Cabinet: America's sense of humor blunted by week of shock". Healingwell.com (Sept. 19). {{cite journal}}: Invalid |ref=harv (help)
  • Mason, Bruce Lionel (1998). "E-Texts: The Orality and Literacy Issue Revisited". Oral Traditions. Vol. 13. Columbia, MO: Center for Studies in Oral Tradition. {{cite book}}: Invalid |ref=harv (help)
  • Mintz, Lawrence E. (2008). "Humor and Popular Culture". In Raskin, Victor (ed.). Primer of Humor Research: Humor Research 8. Berlin, New York: Mouton de Gruyter. pp. 281–302. {{cite book}}: Invalid |ref=harv (help)
  • Morreall, John (2008). "Philosophy and Religion". In Raskin, Victor (ed.). Primer of Humor Research: Humor Research 8. Berlin, New York: Mouton de Gruyter. pp. 211–242. {{cite book}}: Invalid |ref=harv (help)
  • Mulder, M.P.; Nijholt, A. (September 2002). "Humour Research: State of the Art" (PDF). University of Twente, Netherlands: Center of Telematics and Information Technology. Retrieved 10 August 2015. {{cite web}}: Invalid |ref=harv (help)CS1 maint: date and year (link)
  • Nilsen, Alleen; Nilsen, Don C. (2008). "Literature and Humor". In Raskin, Victor (ed.). Primer of Humor Research: Humor Research 8. Berlin, New York: Mouton de Gruyter. pp. ???-???. {{cite book}}: Invalid |ref=harv (help)
  • Oring, Elliott (1984). The Jokes of Sigmund Freud: a Study in Humor and Jewish Identity. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania Press. {{cite book}}: Invalid |ref=harv (help)
  • Oring, Elliott (July–September 1987). "Jokes and the Discourse on Disaster". The Journal of American Folklore. 100 (397). The Journal of American Folklore, Vol. 100, No. 397: 276–286. doi:10.2307/540324. JSTOR 540324. {{cite journal}}: Invalid |ref=harv (help)CS1 maint: date and year (link)
  • Oring, Elliott (Spring 2000). "Review of Jokes and Their Relation to Society by Christie Davies". The Journal of American Folklore. 113 (448): 220. {{cite journal}}: Invalid |ref=harv (help)CS1 maint: date and year (link)
  • Oring, Elliott (2008). "Humor in Anthropology and Folklore". In Raskin, Victor (ed.). Primer of Humor Research: Humor Research 8. Berlin, New York: Mouton de Gruyter. pp. 183–210. {{cite book}}: Invalid |ref=harv (help)
  • Preston, Cathy Lynn (1997). "Joke". In Green, Thomas (ed.). Folklore An Encyclopedia of Beliefs, Customs, Tales, Music, and Art. Santa Barbara, CA: ABC-CLIO. {{cite book}}: Invalid |ref=harv (help)
  • Rahkonen, Carl (2000). "No Laughing Matter: The Viola Joke Cycle as Musicians' Folklore". Western Folklore. 59 (1). Western Folklore, Vol. 59, No. 1: 49–63. doi:10.2307/1500468. JSTOR 1500468. {{cite journal}}: Invalid |ref=harv (help)
  • Raskin, Victor (1985). Semantic Mechanisms of Humor. Dordrecht, Boston, Lancaster: D. Reidel. {{cite book}}: Invalid |ref=harv (help)
  • Raskin, Victor, ed. (2008). Primer of Humor Research: Humor Research 8. Berlin, New York: Mouton de Gruyter. {{cite book}}: Invalid |ref=harv (help)
  • Raskin, Victor; Attardo, Salvatore (1991). "Script theory revis(it)ed: joke similarity and joke representation model". Humor - International Journal of Humor Research. 4 (3–4). Berlin, New York: Mouton de Gruyter: 293–348. {{cite journal}}: Invalid |ref=harv (help)
  • Ruch, Willibald (2008). "Psychology of humor". In Raskin, Victor (ed.). Primer of Humor Research: Humor Research 8. Berlin, New York: Mouton de Gruyter. pp. 17–100. {{cite book}}: Invalid |ref=harv (help)
  • Sacks, Harvey (1974). "An Analysis of the Course of a Joke's telling in Conversation". In Bauman, Richard; Sherzer, Joel (eds.). Explorations in the Ethnography of Speaking. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. pp. 337–353. {{cite book}}: Invalid |ref=harv (help)
  • Shultz, Thomas R. (1976). "A cognitive-developmental analysis of humour". Humour and Laughter: Theory, Research and Applications. London: John Wiley: 11–36. {{cite journal}}: Invalid |ref=harv (help)

Further Sources

List of humor research publications