[go: up one dir, main page]

Hostile attribution bias

Hostile attribution bias, or hostile attribution of intent, is the tendency to interpret others' behaviors as having hostile intent, even when the behavior is ambiguous or benign.[1][2][3] For example, a person with high levels of hostile attribution bias might see two people laughing and immediately interpret this behavior as two people laughing about them, even though the behavior was ambiguous and may have been benign.

The term "hostile attribution bias" was first coined in 1980 by Nasby, Hayden, and DePaulo who noticed, along with several other key pioneers in this research area (e.g., Kenneth A. Dodge), that a subgroup of children tend to attribute hostile intent to ambiguous social situations more often than other children.[1][2] Since then, hostile attribution bias has been conceptualized as a bias of social information processing (similar to other attribution biases), including the way individuals perceive, interpret, and select responses to situations.[4][5] While occasional hostile attribution bias is normative (particularly for younger children), researchers have found that individuals who exhibit consistent and high levels of hostile attribution bias across development are much more likely to engage in aggressive behavior (e.g., hitting/fighting, reacting violently, verbal or relational aggression) toward others.[3][6]

In addition, hostile attribution bias is hypothesized to be one important pathway through which other risk factors, such as peer rejection or harsh parenting behavior, lead to aggression. For example, children exposed to peer teasing at school or child abuse at home are much more likely to develop high levels of hostile attribution bias, which then lead them to behave aggressively at school and/or at home. Thus, in addition to partially explaining one way aggression develops, hostile attribution bias also represents a target for the intervention and prevention of aggressive behaviors.[3]

History

edit

The term hostile attribution bias first emerged in 1980 when researchers began noticing that some children, particularly aggressive and/or rejected children, tended to interpret social situations differently compared to other children.[1][2] For example, Nasby and colleagues presented photographs of people to a group of aggressive adolescent boys (aged 10–16) and observed that a subgroup of these youth exhibited a consistent tendency to attribute hostile intent to the photographs, even when the cues were ambiguous or benign.[1] Similarly, Kenneth A. Dodge and colleagues conducted a study on a sample of school-aged children between 3rd–5th grade and found that children who were rejected were much more likely than other children to exhibit hostile attributions of intent to ambiguous social situations (e.g., when a behavior could have been either accidental or intentional).[2] Furthermore, Dodge and colleagues found that children with high hostile attribution bias then went on to exhibit the most aggressive behaviors later on.

Early studies investigating links between hostile attribution bias and aggression were somewhat mixed, with some studies reporting no significant effects[7][8] or small effects[2][4] and other studies reporting large effects.[9][10] Since then, over 100 studies and a meta-analysis[6] have documented a robust association between hostile attribution bias and aggressive behavior across various samples ranging in age, gender, race, countries, and clinical populations.[3]

Theoretical formulation

edit

Hostile attribution bias is typically conceptualized within a social information processing framework, in which social information (e.g., during an interaction) is processed in a series of steps that leads to a behavioral reaction.[4][5] Accurate social information processing requires a person to engage in six steps that occur in order.[11]

  1. Accurately encode information in the brain and store it in short-term memory. During this step, an individual will pay attention to and code specific stimuli/cues in their environment, including external factors (e.g., someone bumping into you; other people's reactions to the situation) and internal factors (e.g., your affective reaction to the situation).
  2. Accurately interpret or give meaning to encoded information. During this step, an individual may decide if a behavior or situation was meant to be hostile or benign.
  3. Decide a goal for the interaction
  4. Generate potential responses
  5. Evaluate potential responses and select the "optimal" response
  6. Enact chosen response

Hostile attribution bias is theorized to result from deviations in any of these steps,[4] including paying attention to and encoding biased information (e.g., only paying attention to cues suggestive of hostility), biases toward negative interpretations of social interactions (e.g., more likely to interpret situation as hostile), limited ability to generate a broad range of potential responses, and difficulty appropriately evaluating responses and selecting an optimal response. Furthermore, biases in any of the steps affect the rest of the steps.[5] Hostile attribution bias has been particularly linked to step 2 of social information processing (i.e., interpretation of information), but is linked to impairments in other steps as well, including inaccurate perception/encoding of social situations and problems with generating a broad range of potential behavioral responses. For example, a child with high levels of hostile attribution bias may generate fewer potential responses than other children, and these responses may be limited to hostile or ineffective responses to a situation.[12]

Dodge theorized that hostile attribution bias arises from an individual's hostile schemas about the world that are formed through an interaction between a child's neural dispositions and his/her early exposures to hostile socialization experiences.[3] These experiences may include disrupted parental attachment, child abuse, exposure to family violence, peer rejection or victimization, and community violence.

Measurement

edit

In research settings, hostile attribution bias is typically measured with a laboratory task, in which participants are presented with staged interaction (live actors), video, picture, audio, or written presentations of ambiguous social situations. For example, an ambiguous social situation presented might be a video of a child opening a door, causing the door to knock over a tower of toys that another child was building. After the stimulus is presented, participants would be asked to make attributions about the intent of the actor (i.e., hostile vs. benign). (For example: "Do you think the girl who opened the door was trying to be mean, nice, or could have been mean or nice?"). Multiple trials are administered with various ambiguous scenarios, and these attributions are then used by the researchers to determine the level of the child's hostile attribution bias. Careful selection of stimuli and comparison of stimuli across mediums is helpful for accurately assessing an individual's level of hostile attribution bias. A meta-analysis investigating the link between hostile attribution bias and aggressive behavior found that the strongest effect sizes were linked with actual staging of social interactions, followed by audio presentation of stimuli, then video and picture presentation.[6]

Implications

edit

Aggression

edit

Substantial literature has documented a robust association between hostile attribution bias and aggression in youth.[3][6] Hostile attribution bias is traditionally associated with overt physical aggression (e.g., hitting, fighting), such that higher levels of hostile attribution bias predict more aggressive behavior. In particular, much evidence suggests that hostile attribution bias is especially linked to "reactive aggression" (i.e., impulsive and "hot-blooded" aggression that reflects an angry retaliation to perceived provocation) rather than "proactive aggression" (i.e., unprovoked, planned/instrumental, or "cold-blooded" aggression).[13] Beyond physical aggression, elevated hostile attribution bias is also associated with increased use of relational aggression (e.g., gossip, spreading rumors, social exclusion).[14][15][16][17] This is particularly the case when youth attribute hostile intent to ambiguous relational situations (e.g., not receiving an invitation to a party or not receiving a response to a text).[18]

Negative adult outcomes

edit

Hostile attribution bias has also been documented in adult populations, and adults with high levels of hostile attribution bias are over four times more likely to die by the age of 50 than adults with low levels of hostile attribution bias.[19] Hostile attribution bias is particularly linked to relational problems in adulthood, including marital conflict/violence[20] and marital/relationship dissatisfaction.[21] Finally, parents with high levels of hostile attribution bias are also much more likely to use harsh discipline and aggressive parenting,[22][23] which may further contribute to the intergenerational continuity in hostile attribution bias and aggression across time.[24]

Clinical implications for intervention

edit

Hostile attribution bias has been tested as a malleable target for intervention for aggressive behaviors in youth, including in cognitive interventions designed to increase accurate identification of others' intentions and attribution of benign intentions.[25][26][27] Relative success has been documented from these interventions in changing levels of hostile attribution bias, although actual enduring changes in aggressive behavior have been modest.[3]

See also

edit

References

edit
  1. ^ a b c d Nasby, W.; Hayden, B.; DePaulo, B. M. (June 1980). "Attributional bias among aggressive boys to interpret unambiguous social stimuli as displays of hostility". Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 89 (3): 459–468. doi:10.1037/0021-843x.89.3.459. ISSN 0021-843X. PMID 7410713.
  2. ^ a b c d e Dodge, Kenneth A. (1980). "Social Cognition and Children's Aggressive Behavior". Child Development. 51 (1): 162–170. doi:10.2307/1129603. ISSN 0009-3920. JSTOR 1129603. PMID 7363732.
  3. ^ a b c d e f g Dodge, Kenneth A. (2006). "Translational science in action: hostile attributional style and the development of aggressive behavior problems". Development and Psychopathology. 18 (3): 791–814. doi:10.1017/s0954579406060391. ISSN 0954-5794. PMC 2745254. PMID 17152401.
  4. ^ a b c d Perlmutter, M., ed. (2014-01-14). "A Social Information Processing Model of Social Competence in Children". Cognitive Perspectives on Children's Social and Behavioral Development (0 ed.). Psychology Press. pp. 85–134. doi:10.4324/9781315802343-7. ISBN 978-1-315-80234-3.
  5. ^ a b c Crick, Nicki R.; Dodge, Kenneth A. (January 1994). "A review and reformulation of social information-processing mechanisms in children's social adjustment". Psychological Bulletin. 115 (1): 74–101. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.115.1.74. ISSN 1939-1455.
  6. ^ a b c d Orobio de Castro, Bram; Veerman, Jan W.; Koops, Willem; Bosch, Joop D.; Monshouwer, Heidi J. (2002). "Hostile attribution of intent and aggressive behavior: a meta-analysis". Child Development. 73 (3): 916–934. doi:10.1111/1467-8624.00447. ISSN 0009-3920. PMID 12038560.
  7. ^ Dodge, K. A.; Lochman, J. E.; Harnish, J. D.; Bates, J. E.; Pettit, G. S. (February 1997). "Reactive and proactive aggression in school children and psychiatrically impaired chronically assaultive youth". Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 106 (1): 37–51. doi:10.1037/0021-843x.106.1.37. ISSN 0021-843X. PMID 9103716.
  8. ^ Matthys, Waitek; Cuperus, Juliane M.; Engeland, Herman Van (March 1999). "Deficient Social Problem-Solving in Boys With ODD/CD, With ADHD, and With Both Disorders". Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry. 38 (3): 311–321. doi:10.1097/00004583-199903000-00019.
  9. ^ Graham, Sandra; Hudley, Cynthia; Williams, Estella (July 1992). "Attributional and emotional determinants of aggression among African-American and Latino young adolescents". Developmental Psychology. 28 (4): 731–740. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.28.4.731. ISSN 1939-0599.
  10. ^ Waldman, Irwin D. (June 1996). "Aggressive Boys' Hostile Perceptual and Response Biases: The Role of Attention and Impulsivity". Child Development. 67 (3): 1015–1033. doi:10.2307/1131877. JSTOR 1131877. PMID 8706507.
  11. ^ Ostrov, Jamie M.; Godleski, Stephanie A. (January 2010). "Toward an integrated gender-linked model of aggression subtypes in early and middle childhood". Psychological Review. 117 (1): 233–242. doi:10.1037/a0018070. ISSN 1939-1471. PMID 20063970.
  12. ^ Richard, B. A.; Dodge, K. A. (April 1982). "Social maladjustment and problem solving in school-aged children". Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 50 (2): 226–233. doi:10.1037/0022-006x.50.2.226. ISSN 0022-006X. PMID 7069028.
  13. ^ Crick, Nicki R.; Dodge, Kenneth A. (June 1996). "Social Information-Processing Mechanisms in Reactive and Proactive Aggression". Child Development. 67 (3): 993. doi:10.2307/1131875. JSTOR 1131875. PMID 8706540.
  14. ^ Crick, Nicki R. (October 1996). "The Role of Overt Aggression, Relational Aggression, and Prosocial Behavior in the Prediction of Children's Future Social Adjustment". Child Development. 67 (5): 2317–2327. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.1996.tb01859.x. ISSN 0009-3920. PMID 9022243.
  15. ^ Crick, Nicki R.; Grotpeter, Jennifer K.; Bigbee, Maureen A. (2002). "Relationally and physically aggressive children's intent attributions and feelings of distress for relational and instrumental peer provocations". Child Development. 73 (4): 1134–1142. doi:10.1111/1467-8624.00462. ISSN 0009-3920. PMID 12146738.
  16. ^ Crick, Nicki R.; Werner, Nicole E. (December 1998). "Response Decision Processes in Relational and Overt Aggression". Child Development. 69 (6): 1630–1639. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.1998.tb06181.x. ISSN 0009-3920. PMID 9914643.
  17. ^ Werner, Nicole E. (2012). "Do hostile attribution biases in children and parents predict relationally aggressive behavior?". The Journal of Genetic Psychology. 173 (3): 221–245. doi:10.1080/00221325.2011.600357. ISSN 0022-1325. PMID 22919890.
  18. ^ Bailey, Christopher A.; Ostrov, Jamie M. (July 2008). "Differentiating Forms and Functions of Aggression in Emerging Adults: Associations with Hostile Attribution Biases and Normative Beliefs". Journal of Youth and Adolescence. 37 (6): 713–722. doi:10.1007/s10964-007-9211-5. ISSN 0047-2891.
  19. ^ Barefoot, J. C.; Dodge, K. A.; Peterson, B. L.; Dahlstrom, W. G.; Williams, R. B. (1989). "The Cook-Medley hostility scale: item content and ability to predict survival". Psychosomatic Medicine. 51 (1): 46–57. doi:10.1097/00006842-198901000-00005. ISSN 0033-3174. PMID 2928460.
  20. ^ Holtzworth-Munroe, A.; Hutchinson, G. (May 1993). "Attributing negative intent to wife behavior: the attributions of maritally violent versus nonviolent men". Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 102 (2): 206–211. doi:10.1037/0021-843x.102.2.206. ISSN 0021-843X. PMID 8315133.
  21. ^ Bradbury, T. N.; Fincham, F. D. (January 1990). "Attributions in marriage: review and critique". Psychological Bulletin. 107 (1): 3–33. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.107.1.3. ISSN 0033-2909. PMID 2404292.
  22. ^ MacBrayer, Elizabeth Kirby; Milich, Richard; Hundley, Mary (November 2003). "Attributional biases in aggressive children and their mothers". Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 112 (4): 698–708. doi:10.1037/0021-843X.112.4.598. ISSN 0021-843X. PMID 14674871.
  23. ^ Nix, R. L.; Pinderhughes, E. E.; Dodge, K. A.; Bates, J. E.; Pettit, G. S.; McFadyen-Ketchum, S. A. (1999). "The relation between mothers' hostile attribution tendencies and children's externalizing behavior problems: the mediating role of mothers' harsh discipline practices". Child Development. 70 (4): 896–909. doi:10.1111/1467-8624.00065. ISSN 0009-3920. PMID 10446725.
  24. ^ Berlin, Lisa J.; Appleyard, Karen; Dodge, Kenneth A. (2011). "Intergenerational continuity in child maltreatment: mediating mechanisms and implications for prevention". Child Development. 82 (1): 162–176. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01547.x. ISSN 1467-8624. PMC 3059248. PMID 21291435.
  25. ^ Guerra, Nancy G.; Slaby, Ronald G. (March 1990). "Cognitive mediators of aggression in adolescent offenders: II. Intervention". Developmental Psychology. 26 (2): 269–277. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.26.2.269. ISSN 1939-0599.
  26. ^ Hudley, Cynthia; Graham, Sandra (February 1993). "An Attributional Intervention to Reduce Peer-directed Aggression among African-American Boys". Child Development. 64 (1): 124–138. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.1993.tb02899.x. ISSN 0009-3920. PMID 8436025.
  27. ^ Sukhodolsky, Denis G.; Golub, Arthur; Stone, Erin C.; Orban, Lisa (Winter 2005). "Dismantling anger control training for children: A randomized pilot study of social problem-solving versus social skills training components". Behavior Therapy. 36 (1): 15–23. doi:10.1016/S0005-7894(05)80050-4.