Pages that link to "Q34516444"
Jump to navigation
Jump to search
The following pages link to The inversion effect in biological motion perception: evidence for a "life detector"? (Q34516444):
Displaying 50 items.
- Global processing in amblyopia: a review (Q21129133) (← links)
- Motion perception: a review of developmental changes and the role of early visual experience (Q26781611) (← links)
- Signature movements lead to efficient search for threatening actions (Q27302919) (← links)
- Exploring Biological Motion Processing in Parkinson's Disease Using Temporal Dilation (Q27316956) (← links)
- Integrating biological motion: the role of grouping in the perception of point-light actions (Q27317116) (← links)
- Sight restoration after congenital blindness does not reinstate alpha oscillatory activity in humans (Q27320597) (← links)
- Both physical exercise and progressive muscle relaxation reduce the facing-the-viewer bias in biological motion perception (Q27323275) (← links)
- Impairments of biological motion perception in congenital prosopagnosia (Q27339782) (← links)
- Multisensory origin of the subjective first-person perspective: visual, tactile, and vestibular mechanisms. (Q30455176) (← links)
- A predisposition for biological motion in the newborn baby (Q30481088) (← links)
- Bayesian integration of position and orientation cues in perception of biological and non-biological forms. (Q30572508) (← links)
- The influence of motion quality on responses towards video playback stimuli (Q30664593) (← links)
- A comparison of form processing involved in the perception of biological and nonbiological movements. (Q30827033) (← links)
- Action observation: the less-explored part of higher-order vision (Q30829080) (← links)
- Perceiving Animacy From Deformation and Translation (Q30852015) (← links)
- Charting the typical and atypical development of the social brain (Q33374169) (← links)
- Differential orientation effect in the neural response to interacting biological motion of two agents (Q33435666) (← links)
- The evolution of social orienting: evidence from chicks (Gallus gallus) and human newborns (Q33886236) (← links)
- Comparing biological motion perception in two distinct human societies (Q34110034) (← links)
- Biological motion cues trigger reflexive attentional orienting (Q34258748) (← links)
- What can fish brains tell us about visual perception? (Q34264989) (← links)
- Dog experts' brains distinguish socially relevant body postures similarly in dogs and humans (Q34312202) (← links)
- The cultural evolution of mind reading (Q34425786) (← links)
- Perception of social interactions for spatially scrambled biological motion (Q34535730) (← links)
- Seeing the world topsy-turvy: The primary role of kinematics in biological motion inversion effects (Q34600285) (← links)
- Prenatal exposure to a polychlorinated biphenyl (PCB) congener influences fixation duration on biological motion at 4-months-old: a preliminary study (Q34650839) (← links)
- Perceiving the direction of articulatory motion in point-light actions (Q34744446) (← links)
- Investigating the status of biological stimuli as objects of attention in multiple object tracking (Q34749175) (← links)
- Sensitive perception of a person's direction of walking by 4-year-old children (Q35009226) (← links)
- Action perception is intact in autism spectrum disorder (Q35044761) (← links)
- Biological motion primes the animate/inanimate distinction in infancy (Q35058311) (← links)
- Social visual engagement in infants and toddlers with autism: early developmental transitions and a model of pathogenesis (Q35164435) (← links)
- Perception of biological motion from size-invariant body representations (Q35211732) (← links)
- Life motion signals lengthen perceived temporal duration. (Q35837010) (← links)
- Local and global aspects of biological motion perception in children born at very low birth weight (Q36051249) (← links)
- Impaired face and body perception in developmental prosopagnosia (Q36089362) (← links)
- Social Interactions Receive Priority to Conscious Perception. (Q36100309) (← links)
- Reference repulsion in the categorical perception of biological motion (Q36108998) (← links)
- Do People "Pop Out"? (Q36127176) (← links)
- Healthy older observers cannot use biological-motion point-light information efficiently within 4 m of themselves (Q36362073) (← links)
- Impaired global, and compensatory local, biological motion processing in people with high levels of autistic traits. (Q36785596) (← links)
- Contribution of coherent motion to the perception of biological motion among persons with Schizophrenia (Q37090335) (← links)
- Biological motion distorts size perception (Q37644314) (← links)
- Interactions between auditory and visual semantic stimulus classes: evidence for common processing networks for speech and body actions. (Q38498697) (← links)
- Dopaminergic Modulation of Biological Motion Perception in patients with Parkinson's disease (Q38599077) (← links)
- Using a Kinect sensor to acquire biological motion: Toolbox and evaluation (Q38734864) (← links)
- Night-time pedestrian conspicuity: effects of clothing on drivers' eye movements (Q38955686) (← links)
- Visual gravity influences arm movement planning. (Q39637315) (← links)
- The role of spatial and temporal information in biological motion perception (Q40379997) (← links)
- Gravity Cues Embedded in the Kinematics of Human Motion Are Detected in Form-from-Motion Areas of the Visual System and in Motor-Related Areas (Q41458826) (← links)