Pages that link to "Q56454430"
Jump to navigation
Jump to search
The following pages link to Cognitive representations of semantic categories (Q56454430):
Displaying 50 items.
- Motor adaptation scaled by the difficulty of a secondary cognitive task (Q21144291) (← links)
- Categorization: The View from Animal Cognition (Q26744453) (← links)
- Spaces in the Brain: From Neurons to Meanings (Q28067426) (← links)
- Orienting of attention (Q28282162) (← links)
- The Typicality Ranking Task: A New Method to Derive Typicality Judgments from Children (Q28601018) (← links)
- Reasoning with Vectors: A Continuous Model for Fast Robust Inference (Q28607452) (← links)
- Neurocognitive insights on conceptual knowledge and its breakdown. (Q30356638) (← links)
- Understanding semantic and phonological processing deficits in adults with aphasia: Effects of category and typicality. (Q30366577) (← links)
- The neural and computational bases of semantic cognition. (Q30395594) (← links)
- Direct Exploration of the Role of the Ventral Anterior Temporal Lobe in Semantic Memory: Cortical Stimulation and Local Field Potential Evidence From Subdural Grid Electrodes (Q30402244) (← links)
- Memory for musical tempo: additional evidence that auditory memory is absolute (Q30424833) (← links)
- New approaches to the study of amnesic patients: what can a neurofunctional philosophy and neural network methods offer? (Q30427311) (← links)
- When the Spatial and Ideological Collide: Metaphorical Conflict Shapes Social Perception (Q30488891) (← links)
- Visual crowding and category specific deficits for pictorial stimuli: A neural network model (Q30551335) (← links)
- German norms for semantic typicality, age of acquisition, and concept familiarity (Q30557320) (← links)
- In the absence of animacy: superordinate category structure affects subordinate label verification (Q30560320) (← links)
- Spanish norms for age of acquisition, concept familiarity, lexical frequency, manipulability, typicality, and other variables for 820 words from 14 living/nonliving concepts. (Q30560449) (← links)
- Exemplar by feature applicability matrices and other Dutch normative data for semantic concepts (Q33383732) (← links)
- Behavioral antecedents for handwashing in a low-income urban setting in Bangladesh: an exploratory study (Q33644990) (← links)
- Choice of analysis pathway dramatically affects statistical outcomes in breaking continuous flash suppression (Q33774109) (← links)
- Semantic Typicality Effects in Acquired Dyslexia: Evidence for Semantic Impairment in Deep Dyslexia (Q34011110) (← links)
- Semantic structure in schizophrenia as assessed by the category fluency test: effect of verbal intelligence and age of onset (Q34111564) (← links)
- Visual object understanding (Q34307486) (← links)
- Category typicality effects in episodic memory: testing models of distinctiveness (Q34402734) (← links)
- Cognition is … Fundamentally Cultural (Q34443213) (← links)
- Effect of typicality on online category verification of animate category exemplars in aphasia (Q34511779) (← links)
- Is better beautiful or is beautiful better? Exploring the relationship between beauty and category structure (Q34513784) (← links)
- The Centre for Speech, Language and the Brain (CSLB) concept property norms (Q34544975) (← links)
- Social concepts are represented in the superior anterior temporal cortex (Q34615037) (← links)
- Good exemplars of natural scene categories elicit clearer patterns than bad exemplars but not greater BOLD activity (Q34650462) (← links)
- The parallel distributed processing approach to semantic cognition (Q35097321) (← links)
- The long and winding road to uncertainty: the link between spatial distance and feelings of uncertainty (Q35165355) (← links)
- Appraising the role of visual threat in speeded detection and classification tasks (Q35746549) (← links)
- Prototype Effect and the Persuasiveness of Generalizations (Q36582213) (← links)
- The Nature and Neural Correlates of Semantic Association versus Conceptual Similarity (Q36754026) (← links)
- Toward a process-based framework for classifying personality tests: comment on Meyer and Kurtz (2006). (Q36925973) (← links)
- How types of premises modulate the typicality effect in category-based induction: diverging evidence from the P2, P3, and LPC effects (Q37508218) (← links)
- "Penguins don't fly": An investigation into the effect of typicality on picture naming in people with aphasia (Q37604041) (← links)
- Prototypes, exemplars, and the natural history of categorization (Q37626389) (← links)
- The structure of semantic person memory: evidence from semantic priming in person recognition. (Q37944339) (← links)
- Attribute conditioning: changing attribute-assessments through mere pairings (Q38225397) (← links)
- Estimating the average need of semantic knowledge from distributional semantic models (Q38374005) (← links)
- Strudel: A Corpus‐Based Semantic Model Based on Properties and Types (Q38375493) (← links)
- Semantic richness and word learning in children with autism spectrum disorder. (Q38377223) (← links)
- Feature integration in natural language concepts (Q38377443) (← links)
- Electrophysiological correlates of word comprehension: event-related potential (ERP) and independent component analysis (ICA). (Q38383961) (← links)
- Contrasting patterns of comprehension for superordinate, basic-level, and subordinate names in semantic dementia and aphasic stroke patients (Q38384927) (← links)
- Typicality aids search for an unspecified target, but only in identification and not in attentional guidance (Q38387114) (← links)
- Is there an exemplar theory of concepts? (Q38391411) (← links)
- Sources of error in picture naming under time pressure (Q38395220) (← links)