[go: up one dir, main page]

Predation

(Redirected from Predatory)

Predation is a biological interaction where one organism, the predator, kills and eats another organism, its prey. It is one of a family of common feeding behaviours that includes parasitism and micropredation (which usually do not kill the host) and parasitoidism (which always does, eventually). It is distinct from scavenging on dead prey, though many predators also scavenge; it overlaps with herbivory, as seed predators and destructive frugivores are predators.

Solitary predator: a polar bear feeds on a bearded seal it has killed.
Social predators: meat ants cooperate to feed on a cicada far larger than themselves.

Predators may actively search for or pursue prey or wait for it, often concealed. When prey is detected, the predator assesses whether to attack it. This may involve ambush or pursuit predation, sometimes after stalking the prey. If the attack is successful, the predator kills the prey, removes any inedible parts like the shell or spines, and eats it.

Predators are adapted and often highly specialized for hunting, with acute senses such as vision, hearing, or smell. Many predatory animals, both vertebrate and invertebrate, have sharp claws or jaws to grip, kill, and cut up their prey. Other adaptations include stealth and aggressive mimicry that improve hunting efficiency.

Predation has a powerful selective effect on prey, and the prey develop antipredator adaptations such as warning coloration, alarm calls and other signals, camouflage, mimicry of well-defended species, and defensive spines and chemicals. Sometimes predator and prey find themselves in an evolutionary arms race, a cycle of adaptations and counter-adaptations. Predation has been a major driver of evolution since at least the Cambrian period.

Definition

edit
 
Spider wasps paralyse and eventually kill their hosts, but are considered parasitoids, not predators.

At the most basic level, predators kill and eat other organisms. However, the concept of predation is broad, defined differently in different contexts, and includes a wide variety of feeding methods; moreover, some relationships that result in the prey's death are not necessarily called predation. A parasitoid, such as an ichneumon wasp, lays its eggs in or on its host; the eggs hatch into larvae, which eat the host, and it inevitably dies. Zoologists generally call this a form of parasitism, though conventionally parasites are thought not to kill their hosts. A predator can be defined to differ from a parasitoid in that it has many prey, captured over its lifetime, where a parasitoid's larva has just one, or at least has its food supply provisioned for it on just one occasion.[1][2]

 
Relation of predation to other feeding strategies

There are other difficult and borderline cases. Micropredators are small animals that, like predators, feed entirely on other organisms; they include fleas and mosquitoes that consume blood from living animals, and aphids that consume sap from living plants. However, since they typically do not kill their hosts, they are now often thought of as parasites.[3][4] Animals that graze on phytoplankton or mats of microbes are predators, as they consume and kill their food organisms, while herbivores that browse leaves are not, as their food plants usually survive the assault.[5] When animals eat seeds (seed predation or granivory) or eggs (egg predation), they are consuming entire living organisms, which by definition makes them predators.[6][7][8]

Scavengers, organisms that only eat organisms found already dead, are not predators, but many predators such as the jackal and the hyena scavenge when the opportunity arises.[9][10][5] Among invertebrates, social wasps such as yellowjackets are both hunters and scavengers of other insects.[11]

Taxonomic range

edit
Carnivorous plant: sundew engulfing an insect
Seed predation: mouse eating seeds

While examples of predators among mammals and birds are well known,[12] predators can be found in a broad range of taxa including arthropods. They are common among insects, including mantids, dragonflies, lacewings and scorpionflies. In some species such as the alderfly, only the larvae are predatory (the adults do not eat). Spiders are predatory, as well as other terrestrial invertebrates such as scorpions; centipedes; some mites, snails and slugs; nematodes; and planarian worms.[13] In marine environments, most cnidarians (e.g., jellyfish, hydroids), ctenophora (comb jellies), echinoderms (e.g., sea stars, sea urchins, sand dollars, and sea cucumbers) and flatworms are predatory.[14] Among crustaceans, lobsters, crabs, shrimps and barnacles are predators,[15] and in turn crustaceans are preyed on by nearly all cephalopods (including octopuses, squid and cuttlefish).[16]

 
Paramecium, a predatory ciliate, feeding on bacteria

Seed predation is restricted to mammals, birds, and insects but is found in almost all terrestrial ecosystems.[8][6] Egg predation includes both specialist egg predators such as some colubrid snakes and generalists such as foxes and badgers that opportunistically take eggs when they find them.[17][18][19]

Some plants, like the pitcher plant, the Venus fly trap and the sundew, are carnivorous and consume insects.[12] Methods of predation by plants varies greatly but often involves a food trap, mechanical stimulation, and electrical impulses to eventually catch and consume its prey.[20] Some carnivorous fungi catch nematodes using either active traps in the form of constricting rings, or passive traps with adhesive structures.[21]

Many species of protozoa (eukaryotes) and bacteria (prokaryotes) prey on other microorganisms; the feeding mode is evidently ancient, and evolved many times in both groups.[22][12][23] Among freshwater and marine zooplankton, whether single-celled or multi-cellular, predatory grazing on phytoplankton and smaller zooplankton is common, and found in many species of nanoflagellates, dinoflagellates, ciliates, rotifers, a diverse range of meroplankton animal larvae, and two groups of crustaceans, namely copepods and cladocerans.[24]

Foraging

edit
 
A basic foraging cycle for a predator, with some variations indicated[25]

To feed, a predator must search for, pursue and kill its prey. These actions form a foraging cycle.[26][27] The predator must decide where to look for prey based on its geographical distribution; and once it has located prey, it must assess whether to pursue it or to wait for a better choice. If it chooses pursuit, its physical capabilities determine the mode of pursuit (e.g., ambush or chase).[28][29] Having captured the prey, it may also need to expend energy handling it (e.g., killing it, removing any shell or spines, and ingesting it).[25][26]

edit

Predators have a choice of search modes ranging from sit-and-wait to active or widely foraging.[30][25][31][32] The sit-and-wait method is most suitable if the prey are dense and mobile, and the predator has low energy requirements.[30] Wide foraging expends more energy, and is used when prey is sedentary or sparsely distributed.[28][30] There is a continuum of search modes with intervals between periods of movement ranging from seconds to months. Sharks, sunfish, Insectivorous birds and shrews are almost always moving while web-building spiders, aquatic invertebrates, praying mantises and kestrels rarely move. In between, plovers and other shorebirds, freshwater fish including crappies, and the larvae of coccinellid beetles (ladybirds), alternate between actively searching and scanning the environment.[30]

 
The black-browed albatross regularly flies hundreds of kilometres across the nearly empty ocean to find patches of food.

Prey distributions are often clumped, and predators respond by looking for patches where prey is dense and then searching within patches.[25] Where food is found in patches, such as rare shoals of fish in a nearly empty ocean, the search stage requires the predator to travel for a substantial time, and to expend a significant amount of energy, to locate each food patch.[33] For example, the black-browed albatross regularly makes foraging flights to a range of around 700 kilometres (430 miles), up to a maximum foraging range of 3,000 kilometres (1,860 miles) for breeding birds gathering food for their young.[a][34] With static prey, some predators can learn suitable patch locations and return to them at intervals to feed.[33] The optimal foraging strategy for search has been modelled using the marginal value theorem.[35]

Search patterns often appear random. One such is the Lévy walk, that tends to involve clusters of short steps with occasional long steps. It is a good fit to the behaviour of a wide variety of organisms including bacteria, honeybees, sharks and human hunter-gatherers.[36][37]

Assessment

edit
 
Seven-spot ladybirds select plants of good quality for their aphid prey.

Having found prey, a predator must decide whether to pursue it or keep searching. The decision depends on the costs and benefits involved. A bird foraging for insects spends a lot of time searching but capturing and eating them is quick and easy, so the efficient strategy for the bird is to eat every palatable insect it finds. By contrast, a predator such as a lion or falcon finds its prey easily but capturing it requires a lot of effort. In that case, the predator is more selective.[28]

One of the factors to consider is size. Prey that is too small may not be worth the trouble for the amount of energy it provides. Too large, and it may be too difficult to capture. For example, a mantid captures prey with its forelegs and they are optimized for grabbing prey of a certain size. Mantids are reluctant to attack prey that is far from that size. There is a positive correlation between the size of a predator and its prey.[28]

A predator may assess a patch and decide whether to spend time searching for prey in it.[25] This may involve some knowledge of the preferences of the prey; for example, ladybirds can choose a patch of vegetation suitable for their aphid prey.[38]

Capture

edit

To capture prey, predators have a spectrum of pursuit modes that range from overt chase (pursuit predation) to a sudden strike on nearby prey (ambush predation).[25][39][12] Another strategy in between ambush and pursuit is ballistic interception, where a predator observes and predicts a prey's motion and then launches its attack accordingly.[40]

Ambush

edit
A trapdoor spider waiting in its burrow to ambush its prey

Ambush or sit-and-wait predators are carnivorous animals that capture prey by stealth or surprise. In animals, ambush predation is characterized by the predator's scanning the environment from a concealed position until a prey is spotted, and then rapidly executing a fixed surprise attack.[41][40] Vertebrate ambush predators include frogs, fish such as the angel shark, the northern pike and the eastern frogfish.[40][42][43][44] Among the many invertebrate ambush predators are trapdoor spiders and Australian Crab spiders on land and mantis shrimps in the sea.[41][45][46] Ambush predators often construct a burrow in which to hide, improving concealment at the cost of reducing their field of vision. Some ambush predators also use lures to attract prey within striking range.[40] The capturing movement has to be rapid to trap the prey, given that the attack is not modifiable once launched.[40]

Ballistic interception

edit
 
The chameleon attacks prey by shooting out its tongue.

Ballistic interception is the strategy where a predator observes the movement of a prey, predicts its motion, works out an interception path, and then attacks the prey on that path. This differs from ambush predation in that the predator adjusts its attack according to how the prey is moving.[40] Ballistic interception involves a brief period for planning, giving the prey an opportunity to escape. Some frogs wait until snakes have begun their strike before jumping, reducing the time available to the snake to recalibrate its attack, and maximising the angular adjustment that the snake would need to make to intercept the frog in real time.[40] Ballistic predators include insects such as dragonflies, and vertebrates such as archerfish (attacking with a jet of water), chameleons (attacking with their tongues), and some colubrid snakes.[40]

Pursuit

edit
Humpback whales are lunge feeders, filtering thousands of krill from seawater and swallowing them alive.
Dragonflies, like this common clubtail with captured prey, are invertebrate pursuit predators.

In pursuit predation, predators chase fleeing prey. If the prey flees in a straight line, capture depends only on the predator's being faster than the prey.[40] If the prey manoeuvres by turning as it flees, the predator must react in real time to calculate and follow a new intercept path, such as by parallel navigation, as it closes on the prey.[40] Many pursuit predators use camouflage to approach the prey as close as possible unobserved (stalking) before starting the pursuit.[40] Pursuit predators include terrestrial mammals such as humans, African wild dogs, spotted hyenas and wolves; marine predators such as dolphins, orcas and many predatory fishes, such as tuna;[47][48] predatory birds (raptors) such as falcons; and insects such as dragonflies.[49]

An extreme form of pursuit is endurance or persistence hunting, in which the predator tires out the prey by following it over a long distance, sometimes for hours at a time. The method is used by human hunter-gatherers and by canids such as African wild dogs and domestic hounds. The African wild dog is an extreme persistence predator, tiring out individual prey by following them for many miles at relatively low speed.[50]

A specialised form of pursuit predation is the lunge feeding of baleen whales. These very large marine predators feed on plankton, especially krill, diving and actively swimming into concentrations of plankton, and then taking a huge gulp of water and filtering it through their feathery baleen plates.[51][52]

Pursuit predators may be social, like the lion and wolf that hunt in groups, or solitary.[2]

Handling

edit
Catfish has sharp dorsal and pectoral spines which it holds erect to discourage predators such as herons which swallow prey whole.
Osprey tears its fish prey apart, avoiding dangers such as sharp spines.

Once the predator has captured the prey, it has to handle it: very carefully if the prey is dangerous to eat, such as if it possesses sharp or poisonous spines, as in many prey fish. Some catfish such as the Ictaluridae have spines on the back (dorsal) and belly (pectoral) which lock in the erect position; as the catfish thrashes about when captured, these could pierce the predator's mouth, possibly fatally. Some fish-eating birds like the osprey avoid the danger of spines by tearing up their prey before eating it.[53]

Solitary versus social predation

edit

In social predation, a group of predators cooperates to kill prey. This makes it possible to kill creatures larger than those they could overpower singly; for example, hyenas, and wolves collaborate to catch and kill herbivores as large as buffalo, and lions even hunt elephants.[54][55][56] It can also make prey more readily available through strategies like flushing of prey and herding it into a smaller area. For example, when mixed flocks of birds forage, the birds in front flush out insects that are caught by the birds behind. Spinner dolphins form a circle around a school of fish and move inwards, concentrating the fish by a factor of 200.[57] By hunting socially chimpanzees can catch colobus monkeys that would readily escape an individual hunter, while cooperating Harris hawks can trap rabbits.[54][58]

 
Wolves, social predators, cooperate to hunt and kill bison.

Predators of different species sometimes cooperate to catch prey. In coral reefs, when fish such as the grouper and coral trout spot prey that is inaccessible to them, they signal to giant moray eels, Napoleon wrasses or octopuses. These predators are able to access small crevices and flush out the prey.[59][60] Killer whales have been known to help whalers hunt baleen whales.[61]

Social hunting allows predators to tackle a wider range of prey, but at the risk of competition for the captured food. Solitary predators have more chance of eating what they catch, at the price of increased expenditure of energy to catch it, and increased risk that the prey will escape.[62][63] Ambush predators are often solitary to reduce the risk of becoming prey themselves.[64] Of 245 terrestrial members of the Carnivora (the group that includes the cats, dogs, and bears), 177 are solitary; and 35 of the 37 wild cats are solitary,[65] including the cougar and cheetah.[62][2] However, the solitary cougar does allow other cougars to share in a kill,[66] and the coyote can be either solitary or social.[67] Other solitary predators include the northern pike,[68] wolf spiders and all the thousands of species of solitary wasps among arthropods,[69][70] and many microorganisms and zooplankton.[22][71]

Specialization

edit

Physical adaptations

edit

Under the pressure of natural selection, predators have evolved a variety of physical adaptations for detecting, catching, killing, and digesting prey. These include speed, agility, stealth, sharp senses, claws, teeth, filters, and suitable digestive systems.[72]

For detecting prey, predators have well-developed vision, smell, or hearing.[12] Predators as diverse as owls and jumping spiders have forward-facing eyes, providing accurate binocular vision over a relatively narrow field of view, whereas prey animals often have less acute all-round vision. Animals such as foxes can smell their prey even when it is concealed under 2 feet (60 cm) of snow or earth. Many predators have acute hearing, and some such as echolocating bats hunt exclusively by active or passive use of sound.[73]

Predators including big cats, birds of prey, and ants share powerful jaws, sharp teeth, or claws which they use to seize and kill their prey. Some predators such as snakes and fish-eating birds like herons and cormorants swallow their prey whole; some snakes can unhinge their jaws to allow them to swallow large prey, while fish-eating birds have long spear-like beaks that they use to stab and grip fast-moving and slippery prey.[73] Fish and other predators have developed the ability to crush or open the armoured shells of molluscs.[74]

Many predators are powerfully built and can catch and kill animals larger than themselves; this applies as much to small predators such as ants and shrews as to big and visibly muscular carnivores like the cougar and lion.[73][2][75]

Diet and behaviour

edit
Platydemus manokwari, a specialist flatworm predator of land snails, attacking a snail
Size-selective predation: a lioness attacking a Cape buffalo, over twice her weight. Lions can attack much larger prey, including elephants, but do so much less often.

Predators are often highly specialized in their diet and hunting behaviour; for example, the Eurasian lynx only hunts small ungulates.[76] Others such as leopards are more opportunistic generalists, preying on at least 100 species.[77][78] The specialists may be highly adapted to capturing their preferred prey, whereas generalists may be better able to switch to other prey when a preferred target is scarce. When prey have a clumped (uneven) distribution, the optimal strategy for the predator is predicted to be more specialized as the prey are more conspicuous and can be found more quickly;[79] this appears to be correct for predators of immobile prey, but is doubtful with mobile prey.[80]

In size-selective predation, predators select prey of a certain size.[81] Large prey may prove troublesome for a predator, while small prey might prove hard to find and in any case provide less of a reward. This has led to a correlation between the size of predators and their prey. Size may also act as a refuge for large prey. For example, adult elephants are relatively safe from predation by lions, but juveniles are vulnerable.[82]

Camouflage and mimicry

edit
Striated frogfish uses camouflage and aggressive mimicry in the form of a fishing rod-like lure on its head to attract prey.

Members of the cat family such as the snow leopard (treeless highlands), tiger (grassy plains, reed swamps), ocelot (forest), fishing cat (waterside thickets), and lion (open plains) are camouflaged with coloration and disruptive patterns suiting their habitats.[83]

In aggressive mimicry, certain predators, including insects and fishes, make use of coloration and behaviour to attract prey. Female Photuris fireflies, for example, copy the light signals of other species, thereby attracting male fireflies, which they capture and eat.[84] Flower mantises are ambush predators; camouflaged as flowers, such as orchids, they attract prey and seize it when it is close enough.[85] Frogfishes are extremely well camouflaged, and actively lure their prey to approach using an esca, a bait on the end of a rod-like appendage on the head, which they wave gently to mimic a small animal, gulping the prey in an extremely rapid movement when it is within range.[86]

Venom

edit

Many smaller predators such as the box jellyfish use venom to subdue their prey,[87] and venom can also aid in digestion (as is the case for rattlesnakes and some spiders).[88][89] The marbled sea snake that has adapted to egg predation has atrophied venom glands, and the gene for its three finger toxin contains a mutation (the deletion of two nucleotides) that inactives it. These changes are explained by the fact that its prey does not need to be subdued.[90]

Electric fields

edit
 
An electric ray (Torpediniformes) showing location of electric organ and electrocytes stacked within it

Several groups of predatory fish have the ability to detect, track, and sometimes, as in the electric ray, to incapacitate their prey by sensing and generating electric fields.[91][92][93] The electric organ is derived from modified nerve or muscle tissue.[94]

Physiology

edit

Physiological adaptations to predation include the ability of predatory bacteria to digest the complex peptidoglycan polymer from the cell walls of the bacteria that they prey upon.[23] Carnivorous vertebrates of all five major classes (fishes, amphibians, reptiles, birds, and mammals) have lower relative rates of sugar to amino acid transport than either herbivores or omnivores, presumably because they acquire plenty of amino acids from the animal proteins in their diet.[95]

Antipredator adaptations

edit
Dead leaf mantis's camouflage makes it less visible to both predators and prey.
Syrphid hoverfly misdirects predators by mimicking a wasp, but has no sting.

To counter predation, prey have evolved defences for use at each stage of an attack.[96][12] They can try to avoid detection,[97] such as by using camouflage and mimicry.[98] They can detect predators[99] and warn others of their presence.[100][101] If detected, they can try to avoid being the target of an attack, for example, by signalling that they are toxic or unpalatable,[102][103][104] by signalling that a chase would be unprofitable,[105][106] or by forming groups.[107][108] If they become a target, they can try to fend off the attack with defences such as armour, quills, unpalatability, or mobbing;[109][110][111] and they can often escape an attack in progress by startling the predator,[112][113][114] playing dead, shedding body parts such as tails, or simply fleeing.[115][116]

Coevolution

edit
 
Bats use echolocation to hunt moths at night.

Predators and prey are natural enemies, and many of their adaptations seem designed to counter each other. For example, bats have sophisticated echolocation systems to detect insects and other prey, and insects have developed a variety of defences including the ability to hear the echolocation calls.[117][118] Many pursuit predators that run on land, such as wolves, have evolved long limbs in response to the increased speed of their prey.[119] Their adaptations have been characterized as an evolutionary arms race, an example of the coevolution of two species.[120] In a gene centered view of evolution, the genes of predator and prey can be thought of as competing for the prey's body.[120] However, the "life-dinner" principle of Dawkins and Krebs predicts that this arms race is asymmetric: if a predator fails to catch its prey, it loses its dinner, while if it succeeds, the prey loses its life.[120]

 
Eastern coral snake, itself a predator, is venomous enough to kill predators that attack it, so when they avoid it, this behaviour must be inherited, not learnt.

The metaphor of an arms race implies ever-escalating advances in attack and defence. However, these adaptations come with a cost; for instance, longer legs have an increased risk of breaking,[121] while the specialized tongue of the chameleon, with its ability to act like a projectile, is useless for lapping water, so the chameleon must drink dew off vegetation.[122]

The "life-dinner" principle has been criticized on multiple grounds. The extent of the asymmetry in natural selection depends in part on the heritability of the adaptive traits.[122] Also, if a predator loses enough dinners, it too will lose its life.[121][122] On the other hand, the fitness cost of a given lost dinner is unpredictable, as the predator may quickly find better prey. In addition, most predators are generalists, which reduces the impact of a given prey adaption on a predator. Since specialization is caused by predator-prey coevolution, the rarity of specialists may imply that predator-prey arms races are rare.[122]

It is difficult to determine whether given adaptations are truly the result of coevolution, where a prey adaptation gives rise to a predator adaptation that is countered by further adaptation in the prey. An alternative explanation is escalation, where predators are adapting to competitors, their own predators or dangerous prey.[123] Apparent adaptations to predation may also have arisen for other reasons and then been co-opted for attack or defence. In some of the insects preyed on by bats, hearing evolved before bats appeared and was used to hear signals used for territorial defence and mating.[124] Their hearing evolved in response to bat predation, but the only clear example of reciprocal adaptation in bats is stealth echolocation.[125]

A more symmetric arms race may occur when the prey are dangerous, having spines, quills, toxins or venom that can harm the predator. The predator can respond with avoidance, which in turn drives the evolution of mimicry. Avoidance is not necessarily an evolutionary response as it is generally learned from bad experiences with prey. However, when the prey is capable of killing the predator (as can a coral snake with its venom), there is no opportunity for learning and avoidance must be inherited. Predators can also respond to dangerous prey with counter-adaptations. In western North America, the common garter snake has developed a resistance to the toxin in the skin of the rough-skinned newt.[122]

Role in ecosystems

edit

Predators affect their ecosystems not only directly by eating their own prey, but by indirect means such as reducing predation by other species, or altering the foraging behaviour of a herbivore, as with the biodiversity effect of wolves on riverside vegetation or sea otters on kelp forests. This may explain population dynamics effects such as the cycles observed in lynx and snowshoe hares.[126][127][128]

Trophic level

edit

One way of classifying predators is by trophic level. Carnivores that feed on herbivores are secondary consumers; their predators are tertiary consumers, and so forth.[129] At the top of this food chain are apex predators such as lions.[130] Many predators however eat from multiple levels of the food chain; a carnivore may eat both secondary and tertiary consumers.[131] This means that many predators must contend with intraguild predation, where other predators kill and eat them. For example, coyotes compete with and sometimes kill gray foxes and bobcats.[132]

Trophic transfer

edit

Trophic transfer within an ecosystem refers to the transport of energy and nutrients as a result of predation. Energy passes from one trophic level to the next as predators consume organic matter from another organism's body. Within each transfer, while there are uses of energy, there are also losses of energy.

Marine trophic levels vary depending on locality and the size of the primary producers. There are generally up to six trophic levels in the open ocean, four over continental shelves, and around  three in upwelling zones.[133] For example, a marine habitat with five trophic levels could be represented as follows: Herbivores (feed primarily on phytoplankton); Carnivores (feed primarily on other zooplankton/animals); Detritivores (feed primarily on dead organic matter/detritus; Omnivores (feed on a mixed diet of phyto- and zooplankton and detritus); and Mixotrophs which combine autotrophy (using light energy to grow without intake of any additional organic compounds or nutrients) with heterotrophy (feeding on other plants and animals for energy and nutrients—herbivores, omnivores and carnivores, and detritivores).

Trophic transfer efficiency measures how effectively energy is transferred or passed up through higher trophic levels of the marine food web. As energy moves up the trophic levels, it decreases due to heat, waste, and the natural metabolic processes that occur as predators consume their prey. The result is that only about 10% of the energy at any trophic level is transferred to the next level. This is often referred to as "the 10% rule" which limits the number of trophic levels that an individual ecosystem is capable of supporting.[134]

Biodiversity maintained by apex predation

edit

Predators may increase the biodiversity of communities by preventing a single species from becoming dominant. Such predators are known as keystone species and may have a profound influence on the balance of organisms in a particular ecosystem.[135] Introduction or removal of this predator, or changes in its population density, can have drastic cascading effects on the equilibrium of many other populations in the ecosystem. For example, grazers of a grassland may prevent a single dominant species from taking over.[136]

 
Riparian willow recovery at Blacktail Creek, Yellowstone National Park, after reintroduction of wolves, the local keystone species and apex predator.[137] Left, in 2002; right, in 2015

The elimination of wolves from Yellowstone National Park had profound impacts on the trophic pyramid. In that area, wolves are both keystone species and apex predators. Without predation, herbivores began to over-graze many woody browse species, affecting the area's plant populations. In addition, wolves often kept animals from grazing near streams, protecting the beavers' food sources. The removal of wolves had a direct effect on the beaver population, as their habitat became territory for grazing. Increased browsing on willows and conifers along Blacktail Creek due to a lack of predation caused channel incision because the reduced beaver population was no longer able to slow the water down and keep the soil in place. The predators were thus demonstrated to be of vital importance in the ecosystem.[137]

Population dynamics

edit
A line graph of the number of Canada lynx furs sold to the Hudson's Bay Company on the vertical axis against the numbers of snowshoe hare on the horizontal axis for the period 1845 to 1935 
Numbers of snowshoe hare (Lepus americanus) (yellow background) and Canada lynx (black line, foreground) furs sold to the Hudson's Bay Company from 1845 to 1935

In the absence of predators, the population of a species can grow exponentially until it approaches the carrying capacity of the environment.[138] Predators limit the growth of prey both by consuming them and by changing their behavior.[139] Increases or decreases in the prey population can also lead to increases or decreases in the number of predators, for example, through an increase in the number of young they bear.

Cyclical fluctuations have been seen in populations of predator and prey, often with offsets between the predator and prey cycles. A well-known example is that of the snowshoe hare and lynx. Over a broad span of boreal forests in Alaska and Canada, the hare populations fluctuate in near synchrony with a 10-year period, and the lynx populations fluctuate in response. This was first seen in historical records of animals caught by fur hunters for the Hudson's Bay Company over more than a century.[140][128][141][142]

 
Predator-prey population cycles in a Lotka–Volterra model

A simple model of a system with one species each of predator and prey, the Lotka–Volterra equations, predicts population cycles.[143] However, attempts to reproduce the predictions of this model in the laboratory have often failed; for example, when the protozoan Didinium nasutum is added to a culture containing its prey, Paramecium caudatum, the latter is often driven to extinction.[144]

The Lotka–Volterra equations rely on several simplifying assumptions, and they are structurally unstable, meaning that any change in the equations can stabilize or destabilize the dynamics.[145][146] For example, one assumption is that predators have a linear functional response to prey: the rate of kills increases in proportion to the rate of encounters. If this rate is limited by time spent handling each catch, then prey populations can reach densities above which predators cannot control them.[144] Another assumption is that all prey individuals are identical. In reality, predators tend to select young, weak, and ill individuals, leaving prey populations able to regrow.[147]

Many factors can stabilize predator and prey populations.[148] One example is the presence of multiple predators, particularly generalists that are attracted to a given prey species if it is abundant and look elsewhere if it is not.[149] As a result, population cycles tend to be found in northern temperate and subarctic ecosystems because the food webs are simpler.[150] The snowshoe hare-lynx system is subarctic, but even this involves other predators, including coyotes, goshawks and great horned owls, and the cycle is reinforced by variations in the food available to the hares.[151]

A range of mathematical models have been developed by relaxing the assumptions made in the Lotka–Volterra model; these variously allow animals to have geographic distributions, or to migrate; to have differences between individuals, such as sexes and an age structure, so that only some individuals reproduce; to live in a varying environment, such as with changing seasons;[152][153] and analysing the interactions of more than just two species at once. Such models predict widely differing and often chaotic predator-prey population dynamics.[152][154] The presence of refuge areas, where prey are safe from predators, may enable prey to maintain larger populations but may also destabilize the dynamics.[155][156][157][158]

Evolutionary history

edit

Predation dates from before the rise of commonly recognized carnivores by hundreds of millions (perhaps billions) of years. Predation has evolved repeatedly in different groups of organisms.[5][159] The rise of eukaryotic cells at around 2.7 Gya, the rise of multicellular organisms at about 2 Gya, and the rise of mobile predators (around 600 Mya - 2 Gya, probably around 1 Gya) have all been attributed to early predatory behavior, and many very early remains show evidence of boreholes or other markings attributed to small predator species.[5] It likely triggered major evolutionary transitions including the arrival of cells, eukaryotes, sexual reproduction, multicellularity, increased size, mobility (including insect flight[160]) and armoured shells and exoskeletons.[5]

The earliest predators were microbial organisms, which engulfed or grazed on others. Because the fossil record is poor, these first predators could date back anywhere between 1 and over 2.7 Gya (billion years ago).[5] Predation visibly became important shortly before the Cambrian period—around 550 million years ago—as evidenced by the almost simultaneous development of calcification in animals and algae,[161] and predation-avoiding burrowing. However, predators had been grazing on micro-organisms since at least 1,000 million years ago,[5][162][163] with evidence of selective (rather than random) predation from a similar time.[164]

Auroralumina attenboroughii is an Ediacaran crown-group cnidarian (557–562 mya, some 20 million years before the Cambrian explosion) from Charnwood Forest, England. It is thought to be one of the earliest predatory animals, catching small prey with its nematocysts as modern cnidarians do.[165]

The fossil record demonstrates a long history of interactions between predators and their prey from the Cambrian period onwards, showing for example that some predators drilled through the shells of bivalve and gastropod molluscs, while others ate these organisms by breaking their shells.[166] Among the Cambrian predators were invertebrates like the anomalocaridids with appendages suitable for grabbing prey, large compound eyes and jaws made of a hard material like that in the exoskeleton of an insect.[167] Some of the first fish to have jaws were the armoured and mainly predatory placoderms of the Silurian to Devonian periods, one of which, the 6 m (20 ft) Dunkleosteus, is considered the world's first vertebrate "superpredator", preying upon other predators.[168][169] Insects developed the ability to fly in the Early Carboniferous or Late Devonian, enabling them among other things to escape from predators.[160] Among the largest predators that have ever lived were the theropod dinosaurs such as Tyrannosaurus from the Cretaceous period. They preyed upon herbivorous dinosaurs such as hadrosaurs, ceratopsians and ankylosaurs.[170]

In human society

edit
 
San hunter, Botswana

Practical uses

edit

Humans, as omnivores, are to some extent predatory,[171] using weapons and tools to fish,[172] hunt and trap animals.[173] They also use other predatory species such as dogs, cormorants,[174] and falcons to catch prey for food or for sport.[175] Two mid-sized predators, dogs and cats, are the animals most often kept as pets in western societies.[176][177] Human hunters, including the San of southern Africa, use persistence hunting, a form of pursuit predation where the pursuer may be slower than prey such as a kudu antelope over short distances, but follows it in the midday heat until it is exhausted, a pursuit that can take up to five hours.[178][179]

In biological pest control, predators (and parasitoids) from a pest's natural range are introduced to control populations, at the risk of causing unforeseen problems. Natural predators, provided they do no harm to non-pest species, are an environmentally friendly and sustainable way of reducing damage to crops and an alternative to the use of chemical agents such as pesticides.[180]

Symbolic uses

edit
 
The Capitoline Wolf suckling Romulus and Remus, the mythical founders of Rome

In film, the idea of the predator as a dangerous if humanoid enemy is used in the 1987 science fiction horror action film Predator and its three sequels.[181][182] A terrifying predator, a gigantic Man-eater|man-eating great white shark, is central, too, to Steven Spielberg's 1974 thriller Jaws.[183]

Among poetry on the theme of predation, a predator's consciousness might be explored, such as in Ted Hughes's Pike.[184] The phrase "Nature, red in tooth and claw" from Alfred, Lord Tennyson's 1849 poem "In Memoriam A.H.H." has been interpreted as referring to the struggle between predators and prey.[185]

In mythology and folk fable, predators such as the fox and wolf have mixed reputations.[186] The fox was a symbol of fertility in ancient Greece, but a weather demon in northern Europe, and a creature of the devil in early Christianity; the fox is presented as sly, greedy, and cunning in fables from Aesop onwards.[186] The big bad wolf is known to children in tales such as Little Red Riding Hood, but is a demonic figure in the Icelandic Edda sagas, where the wolf Fenrir appears in the apocalyptic ending of the world.[186] In the Middle Ages, belief spread in werewolves, men transformed into wolves.[186] In ancient Rome, and in ancient Egypt, the wolf was worshipped, the she-wolf appearing in the founding myth of Rome, suckling Romulus and Remus.[186] More recently, in Rudyard Kipling's 1894 The Jungle Book, Mowgli is raised by the wolf pack.[186] Attitudes to large predators in North America, such as wolf, grizzly bear and cougar, have shifted from hostility or ambivalence, accompanied by active persecution, towards positive and protective in the second half of the 20th century.[187]

See also

edit

Notes

edit
  1. ^ A range of 3000 kilometres means a flight distance of at least 6000 kilometres out and back.

References

edit
  1. ^ Gurr, Geoff M.; Wratten, Stephen D.; Snyder, William E. (2012). Biodiversity and Insect Pests: Key Issues for Sustainable Management. John Wiley & Sons. p. 105. ISBN 978-1-118-23185-2.
  2. ^ a b c d Lafferty, K. D.; Kuris, A. M. (2002). "Trophic strategies, animal diversity and body size". Trends Ecol. Evol. 17 (11): 507–513. doi:10.1016/s0169-5347(02)02615-0.
  3. ^ Poulin, Robert; Randhawa, Haseeb S. (February 2015). "Evolution of parasitism along convergent lines: from ecology to genomics". Parasitology. 142 (Suppl 1): S6–S15. doi:10.1017/S0031182013001674. PMC 4413784. PMID 24229807.
  4. ^ Poulin, Robert (2011). "The Many Roads to Parasitism". Advances in Parasitology Volume 74. Vol. 74. pp. 1–40. doi:10.1016/B978-0-12-385897-9.00001-X. ISBN 978-0-12-385897-9. PMID 21295676.
  5. ^ a b c d e f g Bengtson, S. (2002). "Origins and early evolution of predation". In Kowalewski, M.; Kelley, P. H. (eds.). The fossil record of predation. The Paleontological Society Papers 8 (PDF). The Paleontological Society. pp. 289–317. Archived from the original (PDF) on 10 September 2008. Retrieved 23 July 2008.
  6. ^ a b Janzen, D. H. (1971). "Seed Predation by Animals". Annual Review of Ecology and Systematics. 2: 465–492. doi:10.1146/annurev.es.02.110171.002341.
  7. ^ Nilsson, Sven G.; Björkman, Christer; Forslund, Pär; Höglund, Jacob (1985). "Egg predation in forest bird communities on islands and mainland". Oecologia. 66 (4): 511–515. Bibcode:1985Oecol..66..511N. doi:10.1007/BF00379342. PMID 28310791. S2CID 2145031.
  8. ^ a b Hulme, P. E.; Benkman, C. W. (2002). "Granivory". In C. M. Herrera; O. Pellmyr (eds.). Plant animal Interactions: An Evolutionary Approach. Blackwell. pp. 132–154. ISBN 978-0-632-05267-7.
  9. ^ Kane, Adam; Healy, Kevin; Guillerme, Thomas; Ruxton, Graeme D.; Jackson, Andrew L. (2017). "A recipe for scavenging in vertebrates – the natural history of a behaviour". Ecography. 40 (2): 324–334. Bibcode:2017Ecogr..40..324K. doi:10.1111/ecog.02817. hdl:10468/3213. S2CID 56280901.
  10. ^ Kruuk, Hans (1972). The Spotted Hyena: A Study of Predation and Social Behaviour. University of California Press. pp. 107–108. ISBN 978-0226455082.
  11. ^ Schmidt, Justin O. (2009). "Wasps". Encyclopedia of Insects. pp. 1049–1052. doi:10.1016/B978-0-12-374144-8.00275-7. ISBN 978-0-12-374144-8.
  12. ^ a b c d e f Stevens, Alison N. P. (2010). "Predation, Herbivory, and Parasitism". Nature Education Knowledge. 3 (10): 36.
  13. ^ "Predators, parasites and parasitoids". Australian Museum. Retrieved 19 September 2018.
  14. ^ Watanabe, James M. (2007). "Invertebrates, overview". In Denny, Mark W.; Gaines, Steven Dean (eds.). Encyclopedia of tidepools and rocky shores. University of California Press. ISBN 9780520251182.
  15. ^ Phelan, Jay (2009). What Is life? : a guide to biology (Student ed.). W.H. Freeman & Co. p. 432. ISBN 9781429223188.
  16. ^ Villanueva, Roger; Perricone, Valentina; Fiorito, Graziano (17 August 2017). "Cephalopods as Predators: A Short Journey among Behavioral Flexibilities, Adaptions, and Feeding Habits". Frontiers in Physiology. 8: 598. doi:10.3389/fphys.2017.00598. PMC 5563153. PMID 28861006.
  17. ^ Hanssen, Sveinn Are; Erikstad, Kjell Einar (2012). "The long-term consequences of egg predation". Behavioral Ecology. 24 (2): 564–569. doi:10.1093/beheco/ars198.
  18. ^ Pike, David A.; Clark, Rulon W.; Manica, Andrea; Tseng, Hui-Yun; Hsu, Jung-Ya; Huang, Wen-San (26 February 2016). "Surf and turf: predation by egg-eating snakes has led to the evolution of parental care in a terrestrial lizard". Scientific Reports. 6 (1): 22207. Bibcode:2016NatSR...622207P. doi:10.1038/srep22207. PMC 4768160. PMID 26915464.
  19. ^ Ainsworth, Gillian B.; Calladine, John; Martay, Blaise; Park, Kirsty; Redpath, Steve; Wernham, Chris; Wilson, Mark; Young, Juliette (2016). "UNDERSTANDING PREDATION - A review bringing together natural science and local knowledge of recent wild bird population changes and their drivers in Scotland". doi:10.13140/RG.2.1.1014.6960. {{cite journal}}: Cite journal requires |journal= (help)
  20. ^ Hedrich, Rainer; Fukushima, Kenji (20 May 2021). "On the Origin of Carnivory: Molecular Physiology and Evolution of Plants on an Animal Diet". Annual Review of Plant Biology. 72 (1). annurev–arplant–080620-010429. doi:10.1146/annurev-arplant-080620-010429. ISSN 1543-5008. PMID 33434053. S2CID 231595236.
  21. ^ Pramer, D. (1964). "Nematode-trapping fungi". Science. 144 (3617): 382–388. Bibcode:1964Sci...144..382P. doi:10.1126/science.144.3617.382. JSTOR 1713426. PMID 14169325.
  22. ^ a b Velicer, Gregory J.; Mendes-Soares, Helena (2007). "Bacterial predators". Cell. 19 (2): R55–R56. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2008.10.043. PMID 19174136. S2CID 5432036.
  23. ^ a b Jurkevitch, Edouard; Davidov, Yaacov (2006). "Phylogenetic Diversity and Evolution of Predatory Prokaryotes". Predatory Prokaryotes. Springer. pp. 11–56. doi:10.1007/7171_052. ISBN 978-3-540-38577-6.
  24. ^ Hansen, Per Juel; Bjørnsen, Peter Koefoed; Hansen, Benni Winding (1997). "Zooplankton grazing and growth: Scaling within the 2-2,-μm body size range". Limnology and Oceanography. 42 (4): 687–704. Bibcode:1997LimOc..42..687H. doi:10.4319/lo.1997.42.4.0687. summarizes findings from many authors.
  25. ^ a b c d e f Kramer, Donald L. (2001). "Foraging Behavior". Evolutionary Ecology. doi:10.1093/oso/9780195131543.003.0024. ISBN 978-0-19-513154-3.
  26. ^ a b Griffiths, David (November 1980). "Foraging costs and relative prey size". The American Naturalist. 116 (5): 743–752. doi:10.1086/283666. JSTOR 2460632. S2CID 85094710.
  27. ^ Wetzel, Robert G.; Likens, Gene E. (2000). "Predator-Prey Interactions". Limnological Analyses. pp. 257–262. doi:10.1007/978-1-4757-3250-4_17. ISBN 978-1-4419-3186-3.
  28. ^ a b c d Pianka, Eric R. (2011). Evolutionary ecology (7th (eBook) ed.). Eric R. Pianka. pp. 78–83.
  29. ^ MacArthur, Robert H. (1984). "The economics of consumer choice". Geographical ecology : patterns in the distribution of species. Princeton University Press. pp. 59–76. ISBN 9780691023823.
  30. ^ a b c d Bell 2012, pp. 4–5
  31. ^ Eastman, Lucas B.; Thiel, Martin (2015). "Foraging behavior of crustacean predators and scavengers". In Thiel, Martin; Watling, Les (eds.). Lifestyles and feeding biology. Oxford University Press. pp. 535–556. ISBN 9780199797066.
  32. ^ Perry, Gad (January 1999). "The Evolution of Search Modes: Ecological versus Phylogenetic Perspectives". The American Naturalist. 153 (1): 98–109. doi:10.1086/303145. PMID 29578765. S2CID 4334462.
  33. ^ a b Bell 2012, pp. 69–188
  34. ^ Gremillet, D.; Wilson, R. P.; Wanless, S.; Chater, T. (2000). "Black-browed albatrosses, international fisheries and the Patagonian Shelf". Marine Ecology Progress Series. 195: 69–280. Bibcode:2000MEPS..195..269G. doi:10.3354/meps195269.
  35. ^ Charnov, Eric L. (1976). "Optimal foraging, the marginal value theorem". Theoretical Population Biology. 9 (2): 129–136. Bibcode:1976TPBio...9..129C. doi:10.1016/0040-5809(76)90040-x. PMID 1273796.
  36. ^ Reynolds, Andy (September 2015). "Liberating Lévy walk research from the shackles of optimal foraging". Physics of Life Reviews. 14: 59–83. Bibcode:2015PhLRv..14...59R. doi:10.1016/j.plrev.2015.03.002. PMID 25835600.
  37. ^ Buchanan, Mark (5 June 2008). "Ecological modelling: The mathematical mirror to animal nature". Nature. 453 (7196): 714–716. doi:10.1038/453714a. PMID 18528368.
  38. ^ Williams, Amanda C.; Flaxman, Samuel M. (2012). "Can predators assess the quality of their prey's resource?". Animal Behaviour. 83 (4): 883–890. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2012.01.008. S2CID 53172079.
  39. ^ Scharf, Inon; Nulman, Einat; Ovadia, Ofer; Bouskila, Amos (September 2006). "Efficiency evaluation of two competing foraging modes under different conditions". The American Naturalist. 168 (3): 350–357. doi:10.1086/506921. PMID 16947110. S2CID 13809116.
  40. ^ a b c d e f g h i j k Moore, Talia Y.; Biewener, Andrew A. (2015). "Outrun or Outmaneuver: Predator–Prey Interactions as a Model System for Integrating Biomechanical Studies in a Broader Ecological and Evolutionary Context". Integrative and Comparative Biology. 55 (6): 1188–97. doi:10.1093/icb/icv074. PMID 26117833.
  41. ^ a b deVries, M. S.; Murphy, E. A. K.; Patek S. N. (2012). "Strike mechanics of an ambush predator: the spearing mantis shrimp". Journal of Experimental Biology. 215 (Pt 24): 4374–4384. doi:10.1242/jeb.075317. PMID 23175528.
  42. ^ "Cougar". Hinterland Who's Who. Canadian Wildlife Service and Canadian Wildlife Federation. Archived from the original on 18 May 2007. Retrieved 22 May 2007.
  43. ^ "Pikes (Esocidae)" (PDF). Indiana Division of Fish and Wildlife. Retrieved 3 September 2018.
  44. ^ Bray, Dianne. "Eastern Frogfish, Batrachomoeus dubius". Fishes of Australia. Archived from the original on 14 September 2014. Retrieved 14 September 2014.
  45. ^ "Trapdoor spiders". BBC. Retrieved 12 December 2014.
  46. ^ "Trapdoor spider". Arizona-Sonora Desert Museum. 2014. Retrieved 12 December 2014.
  47. ^ Gazda, S. K.; Connor, R. C.; Edgar, R. K.; Cox, F. (2005). "A division of labour with role specialization in group-hunting bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus) off Cedar Key, Florida". Proceedings of the Royal Society. 272 (1559): 135–140. doi:10.1098/rspb.2004.2937. PMC 1634948. PMID 15695203.
  48. ^ Tyus, Harold M. (2011). Ecology and Conservation of Fishes. CRC Press. p. 233. ISBN 978-1-4398-9759-1.
  49. ^ Combes, S. A.; Salcedo, M. K.; Pandit, M. M.; Iwasaki, J. M. (2013). "Capture Success and Efficiency of Dragonflies Pursuing Different Types of Prey". Integrative and Comparative Biology. 53 (5): 787–798. doi:10.1093/icb/ict072. PMID 23784698.
  50. ^ Hubel, Tatjana Y.; Myatt, Julia P.; Jordan, Neil R.; Dewhirst, Oliver P.; McNutt, J. Weldon; Wilson, Alan M. (29 March 2016). "Energy cost and return for hunting in African wild dogs". Nature Communications. 7: 11034. doi:10.1038/ncomms11034. PMC 4820543. PMID 27023457. Cursorial hunting strategies range from one extreme of transient acceleration, power and speed to the other extreme of persistence and endurance with prey being fatigued to facilitate capture.Dogs and humans are considered to rely on endurance rather than outright speed and manoeuvrability for success when hunting cursorially.
  51. ^ Goldbogen, J. A.; Calambokidis, J.; Shadwick, R. E.; Oleson, E. M.; McDonald, M. A.; Hildebrand, J. A. (2006). "Kinematics of foraging dives and lunge-feeding in fin whales". Journal of Experimental Biology. 209 (7): 1231–1244. doi:10.1242/jeb.02135. PMID 16547295. S2CID 17923052.
  52. ^ Sanders, Jon G.; Beichman, Annabel C.; Roman, Joe; Scott, Jarrod J.; Emerson, David; McCarthy, James J.; Girguis, Peter R. (2015). "Baleen whales host a unique gut microbiome with similarities to both carnivores and herbivores". Nature Communications. 6: 8285. Bibcode:2015NatCo...6.8285S. doi:10.1038/ncomms9285. PMC 4595633. PMID 26393325.
  53. ^ Forbes, L. Scott (1989). "Prey Defences and Predator Handling Behaviour: The Dangerous Prey Hypothesis". Oikos. 55 (2): 155–158. Bibcode:1989Oikos..55..155F. doi:10.2307/3565418. JSTOR 3565418.
  54. ^ a b Lang, Stephen D. J.; Farine, Damien R. (2017). "A multidimensional framework for studying social predation strategies". Nature Ecology & Evolution. 1 (9): 1230–1239. Bibcode:2017NatEE...1.1230L. doi:10.1038/s41559-017-0245-0. PMID 29046557.
  55. ^ MacNulty, Daniel R.; Tallian, Aimee; Stahler, Daniel R.; Smith, Douglas W. (12 November 2014). Sueur, Cédric (ed.). "Influence of Group Size on the Success of Wolves Hunting Bison". PLOS ONE. 9 (11): e112884. Bibcode:2014PLoSO...9k2884M. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0112884. PMC 4229308. PMID 25389760.
  56. ^ Power, R. John; Shem Compion, R.X. (April 2009). "Lion Predation on Elephants in the Savuti, Chobe National Park, Botswana". African Zoology. 44 (1): 36–44. doi:10.3377/004.044.0104.
  57. ^ Beauchamp 2012, pp. 7–12
  58. ^ Dawson, James W. (1988). The cooperative breeding system of the Harris' Hawk in Arizona (Masters thesis). hdl:10150/276864.[page needed]
  59. ^ Vail, Alexander L.; Manica, Andrea; Bshary, Redouan (23 April 2013). "Referential gestures in fish collaborative hunting". Nature Communications. 4 (1): 1765. Bibcode:2013NatCo...4.1765V. doi:10.1038/ncomms2781. PMID 23612306.
  60. ^ Yong, Ed (24 April 2013). "Groupers Use Gestures to Recruit Morays For Hunting Team-Ups". National Geographic. Archived from the original on 17 September 2018. Retrieved 17 September 2018.
  61. ^ Toft, Klaus (Producer) (2007). Killers in Eden (DVD documentary). Australian Broadcasting Corporation. Archived from the original on 12 August 2009. ISBN R-105732-9.
  62. ^ a b Bryce, Caleb M.; Wilmers, Christopher C.; Williams, Terrie M. (2017). "Energetics and evasion dynamics of large predators and prey: pumas vs. hounds". PeerJ. 5: e3701. doi:10.7717/peerj.3701. PMC 5563439. PMID 28828280.
  63. ^ Majer, Marija; Holm, Christina; Lubin, Yael; Bilde, Trine (2018). "Cooperative foraging expands dietary niche but does not offset intra-group competition for resources in social spiders". Scientific Reports. 8 (1): 11828. Bibcode:2018NatSR...811828M. doi:10.1038/s41598-018-30199-x. PMC 6081395. PMID 30087391.
  64. ^ "Ambush Predators". Sibley Nature Center. Archived from the original on 2 August 2021. Retrieved 17 September 2018.
  65. ^ Elbroch, L. Mark; Quigley, Howard (10 July 2016). "Social interactions in a solitary carnivore". Current Zoology. 63 (4): 357–362. doi:10.1093/cz/zow080. PMC 5804185. PMID 29491995.
  66. ^ Quenqua, Douglas (11 October 2017). "Solitary Pumas Turn Out to Be Mountain Lions Who Lunch". The New York Times. Retrieved 17 September 2018.
  67. ^ Flores, Dan (2016). Coyote America : a natural and supernatural history. Basic Books. ISBN 978-0465052998.
  68. ^ Stow, Adam; Nyqvist, Marina J.; Gozlan, Rodolphe E.; Cucherousset, Julien; Britton, J. Robert (2012). "Behavioural Syndrome in a Solitary Predator Is Independent of Body Size and Growth Rate". PLOS ONE. 7 (2): e31619. Bibcode:2012PLoSO...731619N. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0031619. PMC 3282768. PMID 22363687.
  69. ^ "How do Spiders Hunt?". American Museum of Natural History. 25 August 2014. Retrieved 5 September 2018.
  70. ^ Weseloh, Ronald M.; Hare, J. Daniel (2009). "Predation/Predatory Insects". Encyclopedia of Insects (Second ed.). pp. 837–839. doi:10.1016/B978-0-12-374144-8.00219-8. ISBN 9780123741448.
  71. ^ "Zooplankton". MarineBio Conservation Society. 17 June 2018. Retrieved 5 September 2018.
  72. ^ Bar-Yam. "Predator-Prey Relationships". New England Complex Systems Institute. Retrieved 7 September 2018.
  73. ^ a b c "Predator & Prey: Adaptations" (PDF). Royal Saskatchewan Museum. 2012. Archived from the original (PDF) on 3 April 2018. Retrieved 19 April 2018.
  74. ^ Vermeij, Geerat J. (1993). Evolution and Escalation: An Ecological History of Life. Princeton University Press. pp. 11 and passim. ISBN 978-0-691-00080-0.
  75. ^ Getz, W. M. (2011). "Biomass transformation webs provide a unified approach to consumer-resource modelling". Ecology Letters. 14 (2): 113–24. Bibcode:2011EcolL..14..113G. doi:10.1111/j.1461-0248.2010.01566.x. PMC 3032891. PMID 21199247.
  76. ^ Sidorovich, Vadim (2011). Analysis of vertebrate predator-prey community: Studies within the European Forest zone in terrains with transitional mixed forest in Belarus. Tesey. p. 426. ISBN 978-985-463-456-2.
  77. ^ Angelici, Francesco M. (2015). Problematic Wildlife: A Cross-Disciplinary Approach. Springer. p. 160. ISBN 978-3-319-22246-2.
  78. ^ Hayward, M. W.; Henschel, P.; O'Brien, J.; Hofmeyr, M.; Balme, G.; Kerley, G.I.H. (2006). "Prey preferences of the leopard (Panthera pardus)". Journal of Zoology. 270 (2): 298–313. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7998.2006.00139.x.
  79. ^ Pulliam, H. Ronald (1974). "On the Theory of Optimal Diets". The American Naturalist. 108 (959): 59–74. doi:10.1086/282885. S2CID 8420787.
  80. ^ Sih, Andrew; Christensen, Bent (2001). "Optimal diet theory: when does it work, and when and why does it fail?". Animal Behaviour. 61 (2): 379–390. doi:10.1006/anbe.2000.1592. S2CID 44045919.
  81. ^ Sprules, W. Gary (1972). "Effects of Size-Selective Predation and Food Competition on High Altitude Zooplankton Communities". Ecology. 53 (3): 375–386. Bibcode:1972Ecol...53..375S. doi:10.2307/1934223. JSTOR 1934223.
  82. ^ Owen-Smith, Norman; Mills, M. G. L. (2008). "Predator-prey size relationships in an African large-mammal food web". Journal of Animal Ecology. 77 (1): 173–183. Bibcode:2008JAnEc..77..173O. doi:10.1111/j.1365-2656.2007.01314.x. hdl:2263/9023. PMID 18177336.
  83. ^ Cott 1940, pp. 12–13
  84. ^ Lloyd J. E. (1965). "Aggressive Mimicry in Photuris: Firefly Femmes Fatales". Science. 149 (3684): 653–654. Bibcode:1965Sci...149..653L. doi:10.1126/science.149.3684.653. PMID 17747574. S2CID 39386614.
  85. ^ Forbes, Peter (2009). Dazzled and Deceived: Mimicry and Camouflage. Yale University Press. p. 134. ISBN 978-0-300-17896-8.
  86. ^ Bester, Cathleen (5 May 2017). "Antennarius striatus". Florida Museum. University of Florida. Retrieved 31 January 2018.
  87. ^ Ruppert, Edward E.; Fox, Richard, S.; Barnes, Robert D. (2004). Invertebrate Zoology, 7th edition. Cengage Learning. pp. 153–154. ISBN 978-81-315-0104-7.{{cite book}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  88. ^ Cetaruk, Edward W. (2005). "Rattlesnakes and Other Crotalids". In Brent, Jeffrey (ed.). Critical care toxicology: diagnosis and management of the critically poisoned patient. Elsevier Health Sciences. p. 1075. ISBN 978-0-8151-4387-1.
  89. ^ Barceloux, Donald G. (2008). Medical Toxicology of Natural Substances: Foods, Fungi, Medicinal Herbs, Plants, and Venomous Animals. Wiley. p. 1028. ISBN 978-0-470-33557-4.
  90. ^ Li, Min; Fry, B.G.; Kini, R. Manjunatha (2005). "Eggs-Only Diet: Its Implications for the Toxin Profile Changes and Ecology of the Marbled Sea Snake (Aipysurus eydouxii)". Journal of Molecular Evolution. 60 (1): 81–89. Bibcode:2005JMolE..60...81L. doi:10.1007/s00239-004-0138-0. PMID 15696370. S2CID 17572816.
  91. ^ Castello, M. E.; A. Rodriguez-Cattaneo; P. A. Aguilera; L. Iribarne; A. C. Pereira; A. A. Caputi (2009). "Waveform generation in the weakly electric fish Gymnotus coropinae (Hoedeman): the electric organ and the electric organ discharge". Journal of Experimental Biology. 212 (9): 1351–1364. doi:10.1242/jeb.022566. PMID 19376956.
  92. ^ Feulner, P. G.; M. Plath; J. Engelmann; F. Kirschbaum; R. Tiedemann (2009). "Electrifying love: electric fish use species-specific discharge for mate recognition". Biology Letters. 5 (2): 225–228. doi:10.1098/rsbl.2008.0566. PMC 2665802. PMID 19033131.
  93. ^ Catania, Kenneth C. (2015). "Electric eels use high-voltage to track fast-moving prey". Nature Communications. 6 (1): 8638. Bibcode:2015NatCo...6.8638C. doi:10.1038/ncomms9638. PMC 4667699. PMID 26485580.
  94. ^ Kramer, Bernd (1996). Electroreception and communication in fishes. Vol. 42. Universität Regensburg. doi:10.5283/epub.2108. ISBN 978-3-437-25038-5.
  95. ^ Karasov, William H.; Diamond, Jared M. (1988). "Interplay between Physiology and Ecology in Digestion". BioScience. 38 (9): 602–611. doi:10.2307/1310825. JSTOR 1310825.
  96. ^ Ruxton, Sherratt & Speed 2004, pp. vii–xii
  97. ^ Caro 2005, pp. 67–114
  98. ^ Merilaita, Sami; Scott-Samuel, Nicholas E.; Cuthill, Innes C. (22 May 2017). "How camouflage works". Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 372 (1724). doi:10.1098/rstb.2016.0341. PMC 5444062. PMID 28533458.
  99. ^ Caro 2005, pp. 13–15
  100. ^ Bergstrom, C. T.; Lachmann, M. (2001). "Alarm calls as costly signals of antipredator vigilance: the watchful babbler game". Animal Behaviour. 61 (3): 535–543. doi:10.1006/anbe.2000.1636. S2CID 2295026.
  101. ^ Getty, T. (2002). "The discriminating babbler meets the optimal diet hawk". Animal Behaviour. 63 (2): 397–402. doi:10.1006/anbe.2001.1890. S2CID 53164940.
  102. ^ Cott 1940, pp. 241–307
  103. ^ Bowers, M. D.; Brown, Irene L.; Wheye, Darryl (1985). "Bird Predation as a Selective Agent in a Butterfly Population". Evolution. 39 (1): 93–103. doi:10.1111/j.1558-5646.1985.tb04082.x. PMID 28563638. S2CID 12031679.
  104. ^ Berenbaum, M. R. (3 January 1995). "The chemistry of defense: theory and practice". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 92 (1): 2–8. Bibcode:1995PNAS...92....2B. doi:10.1073/pnas.92.1.2. PMC 42807. PMID 7816816.
  105. ^ Ruxton, Sherratt & Speed 2004, pp. 70–81
  106. ^ Caro 2005, pp. 663–684
  107. ^ Beauchamp 2012, pp. 83–88
  108. ^ Krause, Jens; Ruxton, Graeme D. (10 October 2002). Living in groups. Oxford University Press. pp. 13–15. ISBN 9780198508182.
  109. ^ Ruxton, Sherratt & Speed 2004, pp. 54–55
  110. ^ Dominey, Wallace J. (1983). "Mobbing in Colonially Nesting Fishes, Especially the Bluegill, Lepomis macrochirus". Copeia. 1983 (4): 1086–1088. doi:10.2307/1445113. JSTOR 1445113.
  111. ^ Brodie, Edmund D. (3 November 2009). "Toxins and venoms". Current Biology. 19 (20): R931–R935. Bibcode:2009CBio...19.R931B. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2009.08.011. PMID 19889364. S2CID 9744565.
  112. ^ Cott 1940, pp. 368–389
  113. ^ Merilaita, Sami; Vallin, Adrian; Kodandaramaiah, Ullasa; et al. (26 July 2011). "Number of eyespots and their intimidating effect on naïve predators in the peacock butterfly". Behavioral Ecology. 22 (6): 1326–1331. doi:10.1093/beheco/arr135.
  114. ^ Cumming, Jeffrey M.; Sinclair, Bradley J.; Triplehorn, Charles A.; Aldryhim, Yousif; Galante, Eduardo; Marcos-Garcia, Ma Angeles; Edmunds, Malcolm; Edmunds, Malcolm; Lounibos, L. Phillip; Frank, J. Howard; Showler, Allan T.; Yu, Simon J.; Capinera, John L.; Heppner, John B.; Philogène, Bernard J. R.; Lapointe, Stephen L.; Capinera, John L.; Capinera, John L.; Nayar, Jai K.; Goettel, Mark S.; Nation, James L.; Heppner, John B.; Negron, Jose F.; Heppner, John B.; Kondratieff, Boris C.; Schöning, Caspar; Stewart, Kenneth W.; Aldryhim, Yousif; Heppner, John B.; Hangay, George (2008). "Deimatic Behavior". Encyclopedia of Entomology. pp. 1173–1174. doi:10.1007/978-1-4020-6359-6_863. ISBN 978-1-4020-6242-1.
  115. ^ Caro 2005, pp. v–xi, 4–5
  116. ^ Caro 2005, p. 413–414
  117. ^ Jacobs & Bastian 2017, p. 4
  118. ^ Barbosa, Pedro; Castellanos, Ignacio (2005). Ecology of predator-prey interactions. Oxford University Press. p. 78. ISBN 9780199874545.
  119. ^ Janis, C. M.; Wilhelm, P. B. (1993). "Were there mammalian pursuit predators in the Tertiary? Dances with wolf avatars". Journal of Mammalian Evolution. 1 (2): 103–125. doi:10.1007/bf01041590. S2CID 22739360.
  120. ^ a b c Dawkins, Richard; Krebs, J. R. (1979). "Arms races between and within species". Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 205 (1161): 489–511. Bibcode:1979RSPSB.205..489D. doi:10.1098/rspb.1979.0081. PMID 42057. S2CID 9695900.
  121. ^ a b Abrams, Peter A. (November 1986). "Adaptive responses of predators to prey and prey to predators: The failure of the arms-race analogy". Evolution. 40 (6): 1229–1247. doi:10.1111/j.1558-5646.1986.tb05747.x. PMID 28563514. S2CID 27317468.
  122. ^ a b c d e Brodie, Edmund D. (July 1999). "Predator-Prey Arms Races". BioScience. 49 (7): 557–568. doi:10.2307/1313476. JSTOR 1313476.
  123. ^ Vermeij, G J (November 1994). "The Evolutionary Interaction Among Species: Selection, Escalation, and Coevolution". Annual Review of Ecology and Systematics. 25 (1): 219–236. doi:10.1146/annurev.es.25.110194.001251.
  124. ^ Jacobs & Bastian 2017, p. 8
  125. ^ Jacobs & Bastian 2017, p. 107
  126. ^ Sheriff, Michael J.; Peacor, Scott D.; Hawlena, Dror; Thaker, Maria; Gaillard, Jean-Michel (2020). "Non-Consumptive Predator Effects on Prey Population Size: A Dearth of Evidence". Journal of Animal Ecology. 89 (6): 1302–1316. Bibcode:2020JAnEc..89.1302S. doi:10.1111/1365-2656.13213. PMID 32215909.
  127. ^ Preisser, Evan L.; Bolnick, Daniel I.; Benard, Michael F. (2005). "Scared to Death? The Effects of Intimidation and Consumption in Predator–Prey Interactions". Ecology. 86 (2): 501–509. Bibcode:2005Ecol...86..501P. doi:10.1890/04-0719. ISSN 0012-9658.
  128. ^ a b Peckarsky, Barbara L.; Abrams, Peter A.; Bolnick, Daniel I.; Dill, Lawrence M.; Grabowski, Jonathan H.; Luttbeg, Barney; Orrock, John L.; Peacor, Scott D.; Preisser, Evan L.; Schmitz, Oswald J.; Trussell, Geoffrey C. (September 2008). "Revisiting the classics: considering nonconsumptive effects in textbook examples of predator–prey interactions". Ecology. 89 (9): 2416–2425. Bibcode:2008Ecol...89.2416P. doi:10.1890/07-1131.1. PMID 18831163.
  129. ^ Lindeman, Raymond L. (1942). "The Trophic-Dynamic Aspect of Ecology". Ecology. 23 (4): 399–417. Bibcode:1942Ecol...23..399L. doi:10.2307/1930126. JSTOR 1930126.
  130. ^ Ordiz, Andrés; Bischof, Richard; Swenson, Jon E. (2013). "Saving large carnivores, but losing the apex predator?". Biological Conservation. 168: 128–133. Bibcode:2013BCons.168..128O. doi:10.1016/j.biocon.2013.09.024. hdl:11250/2492589.
  131. ^ Pimm, S. L.; Lawton, J. H. (1978). "On feeding on more than one trophic level". Nature. 275 (5680): 542–544. Bibcode:1978Natur.275..542P. doi:10.1038/275542a0. S2CID 4161183.
  132. ^ Fedriani, Jose M.; Fuller, Todd K.; Sauvajot, Raymond M.; York, Eric C. (October 2000). "Competition and intraguild predation among three sympatric carnivores". Oecologia. 125 (2): 258–270. Bibcode:2000Oecol.125..258F. doi:10.1007/s004420000448. hdl:10261/54628. PMID 24595837. S2CID 24289407.
  133. ^ Lalli, Carol M.; Parsons, Timothy R. (1997). "Energy Flow and Mineral Cycling". Biological Oceanography: An Introduction. pp. 112–146. doi:10.1016/B978-075063384-0/50061-X. ISBN 978-0-7506-3384-0.
  134. ^ "Energy transfer in ecosystems". National Geographic. 18 February 2023. Retrieved 18 February 2023.
  135. ^ Bond, W. J. (2012). "11. Keystone species". In Schulze, Ernst-Detlef; Mooney, Harold A. (eds.). Biodiversity and Ecosystem Function. Springer. p. 237. ISBN 978-3642580017.
  136. ^ Botkin, D.; Keller, E. (2003). Environmental Science: Earth as a living planet. John Wiley & Sons. p. 2. ISBN 978-0-471-38914-9.
  137. ^ a b Ripple, William J.; Beschta, Robert L. (2004). "Wolves and the Ecology of Fear: Can Predation Risk Structure Ecosystems?". BioScience. 54 (8): 755. doi:10.1641/0006-3568(2004)054[0755:WATEOF]2.0.CO;2.
  138. ^ Neal, Dick (2004). Introduction to population biology. Cambridge University Press. pp. 68–69. ISBN 9780521532235.
  139. ^ Nelson, Erik H.; Matthews, Christopher E.; Rosenheim, Jay A. (July 2004). "Predators Reduce Prey Population Growth by Inducing Changes in Prey Behavior" (PDF). Ecology. 85 (7): 1853–1858. Bibcode:2004Ecol...85.1853N. doi:10.1890/03-3109. JSTOR 3450359.
  140. ^ Krebs, Charles J.; Boonstra, Rudy; Boutin, Stan; Sinclair, A.R.E. (2001). "What Drives the 10-year Cycle of Snowshoe Hares?". BioScience. 51 (1): 25. doi:10.1641/0006-3568(2001)051[0025:WDTYCO]2.0.CO;2. hdl:1807/359.
  141. ^ Krebs, Charley; Myers, Judy (12 July 2014). "The Snowshoe Hare 10-year Cycle – A Cautionary Tale". Ecological rants. University of British Columbia. Retrieved 2 October 2018.
  142. ^ "Predators and their prey". BBC Bitesize. BBC. Retrieved 7 October 2015.
  143. ^ Goel, Narendra S.; Maitra, S. C.; Montroll, E. W. (1971). On the Volterra and Other Non-Linear Models of Interacting Populations. Academic Press. ISBN 978-0122874505.
  144. ^ a b Levin, Simon A.; Carpenter, Stephen R.; Godfray, H. Charles J.; Kinzig, Ann P.; Loreau, Michel; Losos, Jonathan B.; Walker, Brian; Wilcove, David S. (2009). The Princeton guide to ecology. Princeton University Press. pp. 204–209. ISBN 9781400833023.
  145. ^ Murdoch, William W.; Briggs, Cheryl J.; Nisbet, Roger M. (2013). Consumer-resource dynamics. Princeton University Press. p. 39. ISBN 9781400847259.
  146. ^ Nowak, Martin; May, Robert M. (2000). Virus Dynamics : Mathematical Principles of Immunology and Virology. Oxford University Press. p. 8. ISBN 9780191588518.
  147. ^ Genovart, M.; Negre, N.; Tavecchia, G.; Bistuer, A.; Parpal, L.; Oro, D. (2010). "The young, the weak and the sick: evidence of natural selection by predation". PLOS ONE. 5 (3): e9774. Bibcode:2010PLoSO...5.9774G. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0009774. PMC 2841644. PMID 20333305.
  148. ^ Rockwood 2009, p. 281
  149. ^ Rockwood 2009, p. 246
  150. ^ Rockwood 2009, pp. 271–272
  151. ^ Rockwood 2009, p. 272–273
  152. ^ a b Cushing, J. M. (30 March 2005). "Book Review: Mathematics in population biology". Bulletin of the American Mathematical Society. 42 (4): 501–506. doi:10.1090/S0273-0979-05-01055-4.
  153. ^ Thieme, Horst R. (2003). Mathematics in Population Biology. Princeton University Press. ISBN 978-0-691-09291-1.
  154. ^ Kozlov, Vladimir; Vakulenko, Sergey (3 July 2013). "On chaos in Lotka–Volterra systems: an analytical approach". Nonlinearity. 26 (8): 2299–2314. Bibcode:2013Nonli..26.2299K. doi:10.1088/0951-7715/26/8/2299. S2CID 121559550.
  155. ^ Sih, Andrew (1987). "Prey refuges and predator-prey stability". Theoretical Population Biology. 31 (1): 1–12. Bibcode:1987TPBio..31....1S. doi:10.1016/0040-5809(87)90019-0.
  156. ^ McNair, James N (1986). "The effects of refuges on predator-prey interactions: A reconsideration". Theoretical Population Biology. 29 (1): 38–63. Bibcode:1986TPBio..29...38M. doi:10.1016/0040-5809(86)90004-3. PMID 3961711.
  157. ^ Berryman, Alan A.; Hawkins, Bradford A.; Hawkins, Bradford A. (2006). "The refuge as an integrating concept in ecology and evolution". Oikos. 115 (1): 192–196. Bibcode:2006Oikos.115..192B. doi:10.1111/j.0030-1299.2006.15188.x.
  158. ^ Cressman, Ross; Garay, József (2009). "A predator–prey refuge system: Evolutionary stability in ecological systems". Theoretical Population Biology. 76 (4): 248–57. Bibcode:2009TPBio..76..248C. doi:10.1016/j.tpb.2009.08.005. PMID 19751753.
  159. ^ Abrams, P. A. (2000). "The evolution of predator-prey interactions: theory and evidence". Annual Review of Ecology and Systematics. 31: 79–105. doi:10.1146/annurev.ecolsys.31.1.79.
  160. ^ a b Grimaldi, David; Engel, Michael S. (2005). Evolution of the Insects. Cambridge University Press. pp. 155–160. ISBN 978-0-521-82149-0.
  161. ^ Grant, S. W. F.; Knoll, A. H.; Germs, G. J. B. (1991). "Probable Calcified Metaphytes in the Latest Proterozoic Nama Group, Namibia: Origin, Diagenesis, and Implications". Journal of Paleontology. 65 (1): 1–18. Bibcode:1991JPal...65....1G. doi:10.1017/S002233600002014X. JSTOR 1305691. PMID 11538648. S2CID 26792772.
  162. ^ Awramik, S. M. (19 November 1971). "Precambrian columnar stromatolite diversity: Reflection of metazoan appearance". Science. 174 (4011): 825–827. Bibcode:1971Sci...174..825A. doi:10.1126/science.174.4011.825. PMID 17759393. S2CID 2302113.
  163. ^ Stanley, Steven M. (2008). "Predation defeats competition on the seafloor". Paleobiology. 34 (1): 1–21. Bibcode:2008Pbio...34....1S. doi:10.1666/07026.1. S2CID 83713101.
  164. ^ Loron, Corentin C.; Rainbird, Robert H.; Turner, Elizabeth C.; Wilder Greenman, J.; Javaux, Emmanuelle J. (2018). "Implications of selective predation on the macroevolution of eukaryotes: Evidence from Arctic Canada". Emerging Topics in Life Sciences. 2 (2): 247–255. doi:10.1042/ETLS20170153. PMID 32412621. S2CID 92505644.
  165. ^ a b Dunn, F. S.; Kenchington, C. G.; Parry, L. A.; Clark, J. W.; Kendall, R. S.; Wilby, P. R. (25 July 2022). "A crown-group cnidarian from the Ediacaran of Charnwood Forest, UK". Nature Ecology & Evolution. 6 (8): 1095–1104. Bibcode:2022NatEE...6.1095D. doi:10.1038/s41559-022-01807-x. PMC 9349040. PMID 35879540.
  166. ^ Kelley, Patricia (2003). Predator—Prey Interactions in the Fossil Record. Springer. pp. 113–139, 141–176 and passim. ISBN 978-1-4615-0161-9. OCLC 840283264.
  167. ^ Daley, Allison C. (2013). "Anomalocaridids". Current Biology. 23 (19): R860–R861. Bibcode:2013CBio...23.R860D. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2013.07.008. PMID 24112975.
  168. ^ Anderson, P. S. L.; Westneat, M. (2009). "A biomechanical model of feeding kinematics for Dunkleosteus terrelli (Arthrodira, Placodermi)". Paleobiology. 35 (2): 251–269. Bibcode:2009Pbio...35..251A. doi:10.1666/08011.1. S2CID 86203770.
  169. ^ Carr, Robert K. (2010). "Paleoecology of Dunkleosteus Terrelli (Placodermi: Arthrodira)". Kirtlandia. 57: 36–45.
  170. ^ Sampson, Scott D.; Loewen, Mark A. (27 June 2005). "Tyrannosaurus rex from the Upper Cretaceous (Maastrichtian) North Horn Formation of Utah: biogeographic and paleoecologic implications". Journal of Vertebrate Paleontology. 25 (2): 469–472. doi:10.1671/0272-4634(2005)025[0469:TRFTUC]2.0.CO;2. S2CID 131583311.
  171. ^ Darimont, C. T.; Fox, C. H.; Bryan, H. M.; Reimchen, T. E. (20 August 2015). "The unique ecology of human predators". Science. 349 (6250): 858–860. Bibcode:2015Sci...349..858D. doi:10.1126/science.aac4249. PMID 26293961. S2CID 4985359.
  172. ^ Gabriel, Otto; von Brandt, Andres (2005). Fish catching methods of the world. Blackwell. ISBN 978-0-85238-280-6.
  173. ^ Griffin, Emma (2008). Blood Sport: Hunting in Britain Since 1066. Yale University Press. ISBN 978-0300145458.
  174. ^ King, Richard J. (1 October 2013). The Devil's Cormorant: A Natural History. University of New Hampshire Press. p. 9. ISBN 978-1-61168-225-0.
  175. ^ Glasier, Phillip (1998). Falconry and Hawking. Batsford. ISBN 978-0713484076.
  176. ^ Aegerter, James; Fouracre, David; Smith, Graham C. (2017). Olsson, I Anna S (ed.). "A first estimate of the structure and density of the populations of pet cats and dogs across Great Britain". PLOS ONE. 12 (4): e0174709. Bibcode:2017PLoSO..1274709A. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0174709. PMC 5389805. PMID 28403172.
  177. ^ The Humane Society of the United States. "U.S. Pet Ownership Statistics". Retrieved 27 April 2012.
  178. ^ Liebenberg, Louis (2008). "The relevance of persistence hunting to human evolution". Journal of Human Evolution. 55 (6): 1156–1159. Bibcode:2008JHumE..55.1156L. doi:10.1016/j.jhevol.2008.07.004. PMID 18760825.
  179. ^ "Food For Thought" (PDF). The Life of Mammals. British Broadcasting Corporation. 31 October 2002.
  180. ^ Flint, Mary Louise; Dreistadt, Steve H. (1998). Natural Enemies Handbook: The Illustrated Guide to Biological Pest Control. University of California Press. ISBN 978-0-520-21801-7.[page needed]
  181. ^ Johnston, Keith M. (2013). Science Fiction Film: A Critical Introduction. Berg Publishers. p. 98. ISBN 9780857850560.
  182. ^ Newby, Richard (13 May 2018). "Is 'Predator' Finally Getting a Worthy Sequel?". Hollywood Reporter. Retrieved 7 September 2018.
  183. ^ Schatz, Thomas. "The New Hollywood". Movie Blockbusters. p. 25. In: Stringer, Julian (2003). Movie Blockbusters. Routledge. pp. 15–44. ISBN 978-0-415-25608-7.
  184. ^ Davison, Peter (1 December 2002). "Predators and Prey | Selected Poems, 1957–1994 by Ted Hughes". The New York Times. Retrieved 5 October 2018. Hughes's earliest books contained a bewildering profusion of poems between their covers: ... fish and fowl, beasts of the field and forest, vigorous embodiments of predators and prey. Hughes as a student had taken up anthropology, not literature, and he chose to meditate his way into trancelike states of preconsciousness before committing poems to paper. His poems, early or late, enter into the relations of living creatures; they move in close to animal consciousness: The Thought-Fox, Esther's Tomcat, Pike.
  185. ^ Gould, Stephen Jay (1995). "The Tooth and Claw Centennial". Dinosaur in a Haystack. Harmony Books. pp. 63–75. ISBN 978-0517703939.
  186. ^ a b c d e f Wallner, Astrid (18 July 2005). "The role of predators in Mythology". WaldWissen Information for Forest Management. Archived from the original on 5 October 2018. Retrieved 5 October 2018. translated from Wallner, A. (1998) Die Bedeutung der Raubtiere in der Mythologie: Ergebnisse einer Literaturstudie. – Inf.bl. Forsch.bereiches Landsch.ökol. 39: 4–5.
  187. ^ Kellert, Stephen R.; Black, Matthew; Rush, Colleen Reid; Bath, Alistair J. (1996). "Human Culture and Large Carnivore Conservation in North America". Conservation Biology. 10 (4): 977–990. Bibcode:1996ConBi..10..977K. doi:10.1046/j.1523-1739.1996.10040977.x.

Sources

edit
edit