[go: up one dir, main page]

Research into music and emotion seeks to understand the psychological relationship between human affect and music. The field, a branch of music psychology, covers numerous areas of study, including the nature of emotional reactions to music, how characteristics of the listener may determine which emotions are felt, and which components of a musical composition or performance may elicit certain reactions.

Simon Vouet, Saint Cecilia, c. 1626

The research draws upon, and has significant implications for, such areas as philosophy, musicology, music therapy, music theory, and aesthetics, as well as the acts of musical composition and of musical performance like a concert.

Philosophical approaches

edit

Appearance emotionalism

edit

Two of the most influential philosophers in the aesthetics of music are Stephen Davies and Jerrold Levinson.[1][2] Davies calls his view of the expressiveness of emotions in music "appearance emotionalism", which holds that music expresses emotion without feeling it. Stephen Davies was the first to claim that "music is expressive of emotions in virtue of displaying emotion characteristics in appearance."[3] Objects can convey emotion because their structures can contain certain characteristics that resemble emotional expression. He says, "The resemblance that counts most for music's expressiveness ... is between music's temporally unfolding dynamic structure and configurations of human behaviour associated with the expression of emotion."[4] The observer can note emotions from the listener's posture, gait, gestures, attitude, and comportment.[5]

Associations between musical features and emotion differ among individuals. Appearance emotionalism claims many listeners' perceiving associations constitute the expressiveness of music. Which musical features are more commonly associated with which emotions is part of music psychology. Davies says that expressiveness is an objective property of music and not subjective in the sense of being projected into the music by the listener. Music's expressiveness is certainly response-dependent, i.e. it is realized in the listener's judgement. Skilled listeners very similarly attribute emotional expressiveness to a certain piece of music, thereby indicating according to Davies that the expressiveness of music is somewhat objective because if the music lacked expressiveness, then no expression could be projected into it as a reaction to the music.[6]

Process theory

edit

The philosopher Jennifer Robinson assumes the existence of a mutual dependence between cognition and elicitation in her description of "emotions as process, music as process" theory, or process theory. Robinson argues that the process of emotional elicitation begins with an "automatic, immediate response that initiates motor and autonomic activity and prepares us for possible action" causing a process of cognition that may enable listeners to name the felt emotion. This series of events continually exchanges with new, incoming information. Robinson argues that emotions may transform into one another, causing blends, conflicts, and ambiguities that make impede describing with one word the emotional state that one experiences at any given moment; instead, inner feelings are better thought of as the products of multiple emotional streams. Robinson argues that music is a series of simultaneous processes, and that it therefore is an ideal medium for mirroring such more cognitive aspects of emotion as musical themes' desiring resolution or leitmotif's mirrors memory processes. These simultaneous musical processes can reinforce or conflict with each other and thus also express the way one emotion "morphs into another over time".[7][page needed]

Conveying emotion through music

edit

When it comes to understanding how emotion can affect children, a lot of the information we have available to us today comes from Piaget's Theory and Stages of Cognitive Development.[8] Piaget's Theory states:

The ability to perceive emotion in music is known to develop early in childhood, and improve significantly throughout development.[9] The capacity to perceive emotion in music is also subject to cultural influences, and both similarities and differences in emotion perception have been observed in cross-cultural studies.[10][11] Empirical research has looked at which emotions can be conveyed as well as what structural factors in music help contribute to the perceived emotional expression. There are two schools of thought on how we interpret emotion in music. The cognitivists' approach argues that music simply displays an emotion, but does not allow for the personal experience of emotion in the listener. Emotivists argue that music elicits real emotional responses in the listener.[12][13]

It has been argued that the emotion experienced from a piece of music is a multiplicative function of structural features, performance features, listener features, contextual features and extra-musical features of the piece, shown as:

Experienced Emotion = Structural features × Performance features × Listener features × Contextual features × Extra-Musical features

where:

Structural features = Segmental features × Suprasegmental features
Performance features = Performer skill × Performer state
Listener features = Musical expertise × Stable disposition × Current motivation
Contextual features = Location × Event[12]
Extra-musical features = Non-auditory features × Expertise[14]

Structural features

edit

Structural features are divided into two parts, segmental features and suprasegmental features. Segmental features are the individual sounds or tones that make up the music; this includes acoustic structures such as duration, amplitude, and pitch. Suprasegmental features are the foundational structures of a piece, such as melody, tempo and rhythm.[12] There are a number of specific musical features that are highly associated with particular emotions.[15] Within the factors affecting emotional expression in music, tempo is typically regarded as the most important, but a number of other factors, such as mode, loudness, and melody, also influence the emotional valence of the piece.[15]

Structural Feature Definition Associated Emotions
Tempo The speed or pace of a musical piece Fast tempo: excitement, anger. Slow tempo: sadness, serenity.
Mode The type of scale Major tonality: happiness, joy. Minor tonality: sadness.
Loudness The physical strength and amplitude of a sound Intensity, power, or anger
Melody The linear succession of musical tones that the listener perceives as a single entity Complementing harmonies: happiness, relaxation, serenity. Clashing harmonies: excitement, anger, unpleasantness.
Rhythm The regularly recurring pattern or beat of a song Smooth/consistent rhythm: happiness, peace. Rough/irregular rhythm: amusement, uneasiness. Varied rhythm: joy.

Some studies find that perception of basic emotional features are a cultural universal, though people can more easily perceive emotion, and perceive more nuanced emotion, in music from their own culture.[16][17][18] Music without lyrics is unlikely to elicit social emotions like anger, shame, and jealousy; it typically only elicits basic emotions, like happiness and sadness.[19]

Music has a direct connection to emotional states present in human beings. Different musical structures have been found to have a relationship with physiological responses. Research has shown that suprasegmental structures such as tonal space, specifically dissonance, create unpleasant negative emotions in participants. The emotional responses were measured with physiological assessments, such as skin conductance and electromyographic signals (EMG), while participants listened to musical excerpts.[20] Further research on psychophysiological measures pertaining to music were conducted and found similar results; musical structures of rhythmic articulation, accentuation, and tempo were found to correlate strongly with physiological measures, the measured used here included heart rate and respiratory monitors that correlated with self-report questionnaires.[21]

These associations can be innate, learned, or both. Studies on young children and isolated cultures show innate associations for features are similar to a human voice (e.g. low and slow is sad, faster and high is happy). Cross-cultural studies show that associations between major mode vs. minor mode and consonance vs. dissonance are probably learned.[22][23] Although that may still remain a nature vs. nurture debate, seeing as how many nursery rhymes are slow, sad sounding (some with what some could describe as disturbing lyrics, such as in "Rock-a-bye Baby"[24]), and in a minor key, yet have the ability to soothe young children.

Music also affects socially-relevant memories, specifically memories produced by nostalgic musical excerpts (e.g., music from a significant time period in one's life, like music listened to on road trips). Musical structures are more strongly interpreted in certain areas of the brain when the music evokes nostalgia. The interior frontal gyrus, substantia nigra, cerebellum, and insula were all identified to have a stronger correlation with nostalgic music than not.[25] Brain activity is a very individualized concept with many of the musical excerpts having certain effects based on individuals' past life experiences, thus this caveat should be kept in mind when generalizing findings across individuals.

Performance features

edit

Performance features refer to the manner in which a piece of music is executed by the performer(s). These are broken into two categories: performer skills, and performer state. Performer skills are the compound ability and appearance of the performer; including physical appearance, reputation, and technical skills. The performer state is the interpretation, motivation, and stage presence of the performer.[12]

There are plenty of examples when it comes to performance features and how a performer may elicit emotion throughout a performance as well as their capability of eliciting emotion in their audience. Take, for instance, how a performer such as Freddie Mercury[26] performed and elicited emotion. He had the ability to elicit emotion in his audience by his strong ability to show emotion through his performance. When emotions are expressed in a performance, the audience can gain a better understanding of what the music represents. [27]

In relation to the use of minor and major keys, it is widely believed that major keys produce feelings of happiness in the listener and minor keys produces feelings of sadness in the listener, however, when it comes to the performer, although most agree that minor keys often caused sadness and major keys often caused happiness, the same may not be the same for the performer. In an experiment done by Matthew Poon and Michael Shutz in 2015 where they had an orchestra perform pieces composed by Bach and Chopin, minor intervals were sung with facial expressions of happiness and those observing perceived it as being happy. The same happened for the major intervals accompanied with sad facial expressions, which caused the perception of sadness. This proved that visual expressions overpowered the music heard.[28]

Listener features

edit

Listener features refer to the individual and social identity of the listener(s). This includes their personality, age, knowledge of music, and motivation to listen to the music.[12]

One of the listener features would be the mindset of the listener. The listener's mindset while listening to music plays a large part in their emotions when it comes to listening to music. In a study by Japanese researchers at Tokyo University of the Arts, the study states that, " sad music might actually evoke positive emotions as well as sadness and may be a good way of coping."[29] An important quote from Reece Alvarez's article, "Does Sad Music Make Humans Happy?", is, "Emotion experienced by music has no direct danger or harm unlike the emotion experienced in everyday life. Therefore, we can even enjoy unpleasant emotion such as sadness,” the researchers said. “If we suffer from unpleasant emotion evoked through daily life, sad music might be helpful to alleviate negative emotion."[30]

Another listener feature, as stated, would be the personality of the listener. Some people are more moved by music than other, especially those with a background in music or any particular knowledge of music. Some people with personality disorders are reached emotionally more easily through music. As stated in the Journal of Clinical Psychology, "People with personality disorders (PDs) are often difficult to reach emotionally in therapy. As music therapy (MT) provides an entry point to emotions and facilitates contact and communication, it is regularly used with this target group."[31] This is something that is also gone over in the Music Therapy section of the article.

Contextual features

edit

Contextual features are aspects of the performance such as the location and the particular occasion for the performance (i.e., funeral, wedding, dance).[12] Most music that is going to be played at a funeral is going to evoke sad emotions versus music that's played at weddings that are meant to evoke feelings of happiness and excitement. Where music is being played is going to play a part in the emotions the listener is going to feel while listening to the music being played.

Extra-musical features

edit

Extra-musical features refer to extra-musical information detached from auditory music signals, such as the genre or style of music. [14]

These different factors influence expressed emotion at different magnitudes, and their effects are compounded by one another. Thus, experienced emotion is felt to a stronger degree if more factors are present. The order the factors are listed within the model denotes how much weight in the equation they carry. For this reason, the bulk of research has been done in structural features and listener features.[12]

Conflicting cues

edit

Which emotion is perceived is dependent on the context of the piece of music. Past research has argued that opposing emotions like happiness and sadness fall on a bipolar scale, where both cannot be felt at the same time.[32] More recent research has suggested that happiness and sadness are experienced separately, which implies that they can be felt concurrently.[32] One study investigated the latter possibility by having participants listen to computer-manipulated musical excerpts that have mixed cues between tempo and mode.[32] Examples of mix-cue music include a piece with major key and slow tempo, and a minor-chord piece with a fast tempo. Participants then rated the extent to which the piece conveyed happiness or sadness. The results indicated that mixed-cue music conveys both happiness and sadness; however, it remained unclear whether participants perceived happiness and sadness simultaneously or vacillated between these two emotions.[32] A follow-up study was done to examine these possibilities. While listening to mixed or consistent cue music, participants pressed one button when the music conveyed happiness, and another button when it conveyed sadness.[33] The results revealed that subjects pressed both buttons simultaneously during songs with conflicting cues.[33] These findings indicate that listeners can perceive both happiness and sadness concurrently. This has significant implications for how the structural features influence emotion, because when a mix of structural cues is used, a number of emotions may be conveyed.[33]

Specific listener features

edit

Development

edit

Studies indicate that the ability to understand emotional messages in music starts early, and improves throughout child development.[9][15][34] Studies investigating music and emotion in children primarily play a musical excerpt for children and have them look at pictorial expressions of faces. These facial expressions display different emotions and children are asked to select the face that best matches the music's emotional tone.[35][36][37] Studies have shown that children are able to assign specific emotions to pieces of music; however, there is debate regarding the age at which this ability begins.[9][15][34]

Infants
edit

An infant is often exposed to a mother's speech that is musical in nature. It is possible that the motherly singing allows the mother to relay emotional messages to the infant.[38] Infants also tend to prefer positive speech to neutral speech as well as happy music to negative music.[35][38] It has also been posited that listening to their mother's singing may play a role in identity formation.[38] This hypothesis is supported by a study that interviewed adults and asked them to describe musical experiences from their childhood. Findings showed that music was good for developing knowledge of emotions during childhood.[39] When you think about lullabies, they are sung to babies as a way to soothe them, therefore music is able to soothe infants. In the article, "Beyond Twinkle, Twinkle: Using Music with Infants and Toddlers", it is said that, "When adults help babies calm down, they are supporting the development of self-regulation (the ability to manage one’s emotional state and physical needs)."[40]

Pre-school children
edit

These studies have shown that children at the age of 4 are able to begin to distinguish between emotions found in musical excerpts in ways that are similar to adults.[35][36] The ability to distinguish these musical emotions seems to increase with age until adulthood.[37] However, children at the age of 3 were unable to make the distinction between emotions expressed in music through matching a facial expression with the type of emotion found in the music.[36] Some emotions, such as anger and fear, were also found to be harder to distinguish within music.[37][41] However, singing about feelings helps babies and toddlers learn the words to describe their emotional experiences (“If you’re happy and you know it…”). The fact is that music evokes feelings—even when there are no words. One recent study found that babies as young as 5 months old are able, under some conditions, to discriminate between happy and sad musical excerpts (Flom, Gentile, & Pick 2008).[42]

Elementary-age children
edit

In studies with four-year-olds and five-year-olds, they are asked to label musical excerpts with the affective labels "happy", "sad", "angry", and "afraid".[9] Results in one study showed that four-year-olds did not perform above chance with the labels "sad" and "angry", and the five-year-olds did not perform above chance with the label "afraid".[9] A follow-up study found conflicting results, where five-year-olds performed much like adults. However, all ages confused categorizing "angry" and "afraid".[9] Pre-school and elementary-age children listened to twelve short melodies, each in either major or minor mode, and were instructed to choose between four pictures of faces: happy, contented, sad, and angry.[15] All the children, even as young as three years old, performed above chance in assigning positive faces with major mode and negative faces with minor mode.[15]

Personality effects

edit

Different people perceive events differently based upon their individual characteristics. Similarly, the emotions elicited by listening to different types of music seem to be affected by factors such as personality and previous musical training.[43][44][45] People with the personality type of agreeableness have been found to have higher emotional responses to music in general. Stronger sad feelings have also been associated with people with personality types of agreeableness and neuroticism. While some studies have shown that musical training can be correlated with music that evoked mixed feelings[43] as well as higher IQ and test of emotional comprehension scores,[44] other studies refute the claim that musical training affects perception of emotion in music.[41][46] However, musical training may be able to affect emotions in the listener as those who are musically trained are either trained to have (or born naturally ((prodigy[47])) with) an ear for particular components of music that may evoke certain emotions in those with musical training that wouldn't affect those without musical training. It is also worth noting that previous exposure to music can affect later behavioral choices, schoolwork, and social interactions.[48] Therefore, previous music exposure does seem to have an effect on the personality and emotions of a child later in their life, and would subsequently affect their ability to perceive as well as express emotions during exposure to music. Gender, however, has not been shown to lead to a difference in perception of emotions found in music.[41][46] Further research into which factors affect an individual's perception of emotion in music and the ability of the individual to have music-induced emotions are needed.

Eliciting emotion through music

edit

Along with the research that music conveys an emotion to its listener(s), it has also been shown that music can produce emotion in the listener(s).[49] This view often causes debate because the emotion is produced within the listener, and is consequently hard to measure. In spite of controversy, studies have shown observable responses to elicited emotions, which reinforces the Emotivists' view that music does elicit real emotional responses.[9][13]

Responses to elicited emotion

edit

The structural features of music not only help convey an emotional message to the listener, but also may create emotion in the listener.[12] These emotions can be completely new feelings or may be an extension of previous emotional events. Empirical research has shown how listeners can absorb the piece's expression as their own emotion, as well as invoke a unique response based on their personal experiences.[34]

Basic emotions

edit

In research on eliciting emotion, participants report personally feeling a certain emotion in response to hearing a musical piece.[49] Researchers have investigated whether the same structures that conveyed a particular emotion could elicit it as well. The researchers presented excerpts of fast tempo, major mode music and slow tempo, minor tone music to participants; these musical structures were chosen because they are known to convey happiness and sadness respectively.[32] Participants rated their own emotions with elevated levels of happiness after listening to music with structures that convey happiness and elevated sadness after music with structures that convey sadness.[32]

This evidence suggests that the same structures that convey emotions in music can also elicit those same emotions in the listener. In light of this finding, there has been particular controversy about music eliciting negative emotions. Cognitivists argue that choosing to listen to music that elicits negative emotions like sadness would be paradoxical, as listeners would not willingly strive to induce sadness,[13] whereas emotivists purport that music can elicit negative emotions, and listeners knowingly choose to listen in order to feel sadness in an impersonal way, similar to a viewer's desire to watch a tragic film.[13][49] The reasons why people sometimes listen to sad music when feeling sad has been explored by means of interviewing people about their motivations for doing so. As a result of this research, it has been found that people sometimes listen to sad music when feeling sad to intensify feelings of sadness. Other reasons for listening to sad music when feeling sad were in order to retrieve memories, to feel closer to other people, for cognitive reappraisal, to feel befriended by the music, to distract oneself, and for mood enhancement.[50] This goes back to one of the features of listeners, where it was stated that "emotion experienced by music has no direct danger or harm unlike the emotion experienced in everyday life. Therefore, we can even enjoy unpleasant emotion such as sadness,” the researchers said. “If we suffer from unpleasant emotion evoked through daily life, sad music might be helpful to alleviate negative emotion."[51]

This brings us to wonder what other basic emotions are felt by hearing a musical piece. It can be assumed that happy sounding music can make us feel happy, or it might be that if we are happy, we are more likely to listen to music that would evoke feelings of happiness. Just as a person who is suffering from an unpleasant emotion might listen to sadder sounding music to help alleviate the negative emotion, happier people tend to listen to music with a happier tone to it.

Researchers have also found an effect between one's familiarity with a piece of music and the emotions it elicits.[52] One study suggested that familiarity with a piece of music increases the emotions experienced by the listener; half of participants were played twelve random musical excerpts one time, and rated their emotions after each piece. The other half of the participants listened to twelve random excerpts five times, and started their ratings on the third repetition. Findings showed that participants who listened to the excerpts five times rated their emotions with higher intensity than the participants who listened to them only once.[52] This goes to show that the more familiar one is with a piece of music and it's musical components, the stronger the emotional response to it will be.

Emotional memories and actions

edit

Music may not only elicit new emotions, but connect listeners with other emotional sources.[12] Music serves as a powerful cue to recall emotional memories back into awareness.[53] Because music is such a pervasive part of social life, present in weddings, funerals and religious ceremonies, it brings back emotional memories that are often already associated with it.[12][34] Music is also processed by the lower, sensory levels of the brain, making it impervious to later memory distortions. Therefore creating a strong connection between emotion and music within memory makes it easier to recall one when prompted by the other.[12] Music can also tap into empathy, inducing emotions that are assumed to be felt by the performer or composer. Listeners can become sad because they recognize that those emotions must have been felt by the composer,[54][55] much as the viewer of a play can empathize for the actors.

Listeners may also respond to emotional music through action.[12] Throughout history music was composed to inspire people into specific action - to march, dance, sing or fight. Consequently, heightening the emotions in all these events. In fact, many people report being unable to sit still when certain rhythms are played, in some cases even engaging in subliminal actions when physical manifestations should be suppressed.[34] Examples of this can be seen in young children's spontaneous outbursts into motion upon hearing music, or exuberant expressions shown at concerts.[34]

Juslin and Västfjäll's BRECVEM model

edit

Juslin and Västfjäll developed a model of seven ways in which music can elicit emotion, called the BRECVEM model.[56][57]

  • Brain stem reflex: "This refers to a process whereby an emotion is induced by music because one or more fundamental acoustical characteristics of the music are taken by the brain stem to signal a potentially important and urgent event. All other things being equal, sounds that are sudden, loud, dissonant, or feature fast temporal patterns induce arousal or feelings of unpleasantness in listeners...Such responses reflect the impact of auditory sensations – music as sound in the most basic sense."
  • Rhythmic entrainment: "This refers to a process whereby an emotion is evoked by a piece of music because a powerful, external rhythm in the music influences some internal bodily rhythm of the listener (e.g. heart rate), such that the latter rhythm adjusts toward and eventually 'locks in' to a common periodicity. The adjusted heart rate can then spread to other components of emotion such as feeling, through proprioceptive feedback. This may produce an increased level of arousal in the listener."[58]
  • Evaluative conditioning: "This refers to a process whereby an emotion is induced by a piece of music simply because this stimulus has been paired repeatedly with other positive or negative stimuli. Thus, for instance, a particular piece of music may have occurred repeatedly together in time with a specific event that always made you happy (e.g., meeting your best friend). Over time, through repeated pairings, the music will eventually come to evoke happiness even in the absence of the friendly interaction."
  • Emotional contagion: "This refers to a process whereby an emotion is induced by a piece of music because the listener perceives the emotional expression of the music, and then 'mimics' this expression internally, which by means of either peripheral feedback from muscles, or a more direct activation of the relevant emotional representations in the brain, leads to an induction of the same emotion."
  • Visual imagery: "This refers to a process whereby an emotion is induced in a listener because he or she conjures up visual images (e.g., of a beautiful landscape) while listening to the music."
  • Episodic memory: "This refers to a process whereby an emotion is induced in a listener because the music evokes a memory of a particular event in the listener's life. This is sometimes referred to as the 'Darling, they are playing our tune' phenomenon."[59]
  • Musical expectancy: "This refers to a process whereby an emotion is induced in a listener because a specific feature of the music violates, delays, or confirms the listener's expectations about the continuation of the music."

Susino et.al. Framework for Adaptable Musical Emotions - FAME

edit

Susino and colleagues developed the Framework for Adaptable Musical Emotions (FAME),[60] which explains emotional responses to music through the mechanism of emotion adaptability on a continuum of evolutionary to fleeting time frames. FAME represents an advance on models of music and emotion that primarily focus on decoding emotional signals from the sounded music. Instead, the framework explains that emotional responses can be influenced by situational contexts that can be evaluated systematically. The framework also explains the idea that emotional responses to music are formed in an adaptive way to accommodate or assimilate a wide range of cues arising from psychophysical, cultural, and personal variables interacting with situational contexts. The concept of emotion adaptability illustrates how emotional responses continuously shift to align with fluid situational contexts across time. Emotion adaptability helps to explain and predict the convergence and divergence of emotional responses to music across individuals and cultures over evolutionary time frames to fleeting moments. It also enriches the traditional dichotomy between universal and cultural influences on emotional understanding, taking into consideration fluid interactions over time that are dependent on knowledge, experience, and available cues within a situational context.

Musical expectancy

edit

With regards to violations of expectation in music several interesting results have been found. It has for example been found that listening to unconventional music may sometimes cause a meaning threat and result in compensatory behaviour in order to restore meaning.[61] Musical expectancy is defined as a process whereby an emotion is aroused in a listener because a specific feature of the music violates, delays, or confirms the listener's expectations about the continuation of the music. Every time the listener hears a piece of music, he or she has such expectations, based on music he or she has heard before. For example, the sequential progression of E-F# may set up the expectation that the music will continue with G#. In other words, some notes seem to imply other notes; and if these musical implications are not realized — if the listener's expectations are thwarted — an affective response might be induced.[62]

Aesthetic judgement and BRECVEMA

edit

In 2013, Juslin created an additional aspect to the BRECVEM model called aesthetic judgement.[63] This is the criteria which each individual has as a metric for music's aesthetic value. This can involve a number of varying personal preferences, such as the message conveyed, skill presented or novelty of style or idea.

Comparison of conveyed and elicited emotions

edit

Evidence for emotion in music

edit

There has been a bulk of evidence that listeners can identify specific emotions with certain types of music, but there has been less concrete evidence that music may elicit emotions.[12] This is due to the fact that elicited emotion is subjective; and thus, it is difficult to find a valid criterion to study it.[12] However, newer studies have found that music has the capability of affecting the major reaction components of emotion, "including subjective feeling, physiological arousal (autonomic and endocrine changes), motoric expression of emotion (such as smiling) and action tendencies (for example, dancing, singing, playing an instrument, foot tapping and clapping, even if only covertly).[64] Elicited and conveyed emotion in music is usually understood from three types of evidence: self-report, physiological responses, and expressive behavior. Researchers use one or a combination of these methods to investigate emotional reactions to music.[12]

Self-report

edit

The self-report method is a verbal report by the listener regarding what they are experiencing. This is the most widely used method for studying emotion and has shown that people identify emotions and personally experience emotions while listening to music.[12] Research in the area has shown that listeners' emotional responses are highly consistent. In fact, a meta-analysis of 41 studies on music performance found that happiness, sadness, tenderness, threat, and anger were identified above chance by listeners.[65] Another study compared untrained listeners to musically trained listeners.[65] Both groups were required to categorize musical excerpts that conveyed similar emotions. The findings showed that the categorizations were not different between the trained and untrained; thus demonstrating that the untrained listeners are highly accurate in perceiving emotion.[65] It is more difficult to find evidence for elicited emotion, as it depends solely on the subjective response of the listener. This leaves reporting vulnerable to self-report biases such as participants responding according to social prescriptions or responding as they think the experimenter wants them to.[12] As a result, the validity of the self-report method is often questioned, and consequently researchers are reluctant to draw definitive conclusions solely from these reports.[12]

Physiological responses

edit

Emotions are known to create physiological, or bodily, changes in a person, which can be tested experimentally. Some evidence shows one of these changes is within the nervous system.[12] Arousing music is related to increased heart rate and muscle tension; calming music is connected to decreased heart rate and muscle tension, and increased skin temperature.[12] Other research identifies outward physical responses such as shivering or goose bumps to be caused by changes in harmony, while tears and a lump-in-the-throat sensation are provoked by changes in melody.[66] Researchers test these responses through the use of instruments for physiological measurement, such as recording pulse rate.[12] As stated in the NAMI (National Alliance of Mental Illness) website's article, "How Music Can Make Us Feel Good" by Alex Fuentes, "Many bodies of research support the healing effects of music. Our favorite melodies release dopamine, known as the feel-good hormone, which activates our brain’s pleasure and reward system. Music can have a positive, immediate impact on our mental state; fast tempos can psychologically and physiologically arouse us, helping energize us for the day. Slower, meditative tunes can help us to relax and lower our stress levels."[67]

Expressive behavior

edit

People are also known to show outward manifestations of their emotional states while listening to music. Studies using facial electromyography (EMG) have found that people react with subliminal facial expressions when listening to expressive music.[34] In addition, music provides a stimulus for expressive behavior in many social contexts, such as concerts, dances, and ceremonies.[12][34] Although these expressive behaviors can be measured experimentally, there have been very few controlled studies observing this behavior.[12]

Strength of effects

edit

Within the comparison between elicited and conveyed emotions, researchers have examined the relationship between these two types of responses to music. In general, research agrees that feeling and perception ratings are highly correlated, but not identical.[32] More specifically, studies are inconclusive as to whether one response has a stronger effect than the other, and in what ways these two responses relate.[32][52][68]

Conveyed more than elicited

edit

In one study, participants heard a random selection of 24 excerpts, displaying six types of emotions, five times in a row.[52] Half the participants described the emotions the music conveyed, and the other half responded with how the music made them feel. The results found that emotions conveyed by music were more intense than the emotions elicited by the same piece of music.[52] Another study investigated under what specific conditions strong emotions were conveyed. Findings showed that ratings for conveyed emotions were higher in happy responses to music with consistent cues for happiness (i.e., fast tempo and major mode), for sad responses to music with consistent cues for sadness (i.e., slow tempo and minor mode,) and for sad responses in general.[32] These studies suggest that people can recognize the emotion displayed in music more readily than feeling it personally.

Sometimes conveyed, sometimes elicited

edit

Another study that had 32 participants listen to twelve musical pieces and found that the strength of perceived and elicited emotions were dependent on the structures of the piece of music.[68] Perceived emotions were stronger than felt emotions when listeners rated for arousal and positive and negative activation. On the other hand, elicited emotions were stronger than perceived emotions when rating for pleasantness.[68]

Elicited more than conveyed

edit

In another study analysis revealed that emotional responses were stronger than the listeners' perceptions of emotions.[68] This study used a between-subjects design, where 20 listeners judged to what extent they perceived four emotions: happy, sad, peaceful, and scared. A separate 19 listeners rated to what extent they experienced each of these emotions. The findings showed that all music stimuli elicited specific emotions for the group of participants rating elicited emotion, while music stimuli only occasionally conveyed emotion to the participants in the group identifying which emotions the music conveyed.[68] Based on these inconsistent findings, there is much research left to be done in order to determine how conveyed and elicited emotions are similar and different. There is disagreement about whether music induces 'true' emotions or if the emotions reported as felt in studies are instead just participants stating the emotions found in the music they are listening to.[69][70]

Music as a therapeutic tool

edit

Music therapy as a therapeutic tool has been shown to be an effective treatment for various ailments. Therapeutic techniques involve eliciting emotions by listening to music, composing music or lyrics and performing music.[71]

Music therapy sessions may have the ability to help drug users who are attempting to break a drug habit, with users reporting feeling better able to feel emotions without the aid of drug use.[72] Music therapy may also be a viable option for people experiencing extended stays in a hospital due to illness. In one study, music therapy provided child oncology patients with enhanced environmental support elements and elicited more engaging behaviors from the child.[73] When treating troubled teenagers, a study by Keen revealed that music therapy has allowed therapists to interact with teenagers with less resistance, thus facilitating self-expression in the teenager.[citation needed]

Music therapy has also shown great promise in individuals with autism, serving as an emotional outlet for these patients. While other avenues of emotional expression and understanding may be difficult for people with autism, music may provide those with limited understanding of socio-emotional cues a way of accessing emotion [74] and displaying their emotions in a safer and healthier way. Music therapy has been known to be helpful to those struggling with mental illness, children and teens who've experienced trauma, the spectrum of autism, and those with addiction issues by giving them an alternative to standard talk therapy, where these people might struggle to be able to communicate how they feel or what they're going through. Music therapy offers them the ability to express themselves through music, help them to open up to talk about difficult subjects and work through those emotions.[75]

Negative impact from music

edit

The Negative impact of music offers insights into how music can influence collective sentiments, sometimes fostering shared emotions such as fear through its lyrics and melodies. Drawing from philosophical concepts, this section delves into the unique potency of music in shaping group beliefs.[76]

Understanding how music communicates with collective emotions involves examining the constituent elements of a song, its execution, and its resonance with audiences.[77]Through this examination, uncover how music serves as a conduit for expression and emotional elicitation within groups. Fundamental notions such as "cognitive dissonance" and "emotional resonance" are introduced to elucidate the cognitive processes involved when music blends its lyrical and melodic elements to shape collective emotions.  This exploration sheds light on the ways in which music, with its blend of words and melodies, can potentially contribute to shaping group beliefs and eliciting shared fears within the intricate landscape of human emotions.

Lyrics and Emotion

edit

Fear-inducing lyrics are meticulously constructed to evoke feelings of unease or apprehension when heard. They employ specific words and phrases designed to instill doubt or a sense of rejection.

For instance, negative terms like "not" are often utilized, contributing to the creation of fear.[78]Additionally, expressions such as "pass" or "disappear" may be employed to suggest fluctuating emotions, further intensifying the sense of uncertainty. These lyrics frequently depict frightening scenarios through vivid imagery and impactful language that engages the senses.

This serves to immerse listeners in the perceived danger and heightens their emotional response. Sometimes, the delivery of the lyrics or the musical accompaniment enhances the sense of fear, amplifying its impact.[79] Collectively, these strategies collaborate to elicit a potent sensation of fear upon encountering the lyrics. Even after the music ceases, the lingering fear persists in the listener's psyche due to the powerful emotions stirred by the lyrics.[80]

However, lyrics do not only elicit the emotion of fear, but elicit many emotions such as sadness, despair, happiness, excitement, and so on. Lyrics in music even have the ability to persuade the listener to change their behavior, help with interpersonal issues and problems, create discussions between listeners, and inspire the listener to think a little deeper about their own feelings, emotions, problems, and any difficulties that they may be dealing with. Not only do lyrics in music affect one's emotions, but the words that are used can connotate certain emotions. For example, we observed that the perceived valence values were negatively correlated with the usage frequency of words related to negative emotions (e.g., “sad”, “horrible”, “angry”, and “bitter”), tentativeness (e.g., “seem”, “dim”, “guess”, and “dubitation”), insight (e.g., “understanding”, “notice”, “analyze”, and “know”), and exclusiveness (e.g., “exclude”, “forget”, “ignore”, and “cancel”). It is obvious that the usage of emotional words is related to perceived emotions because emotional words shape emotional percepts (Gendron et al., 2012).[81]

Along with the idea of lyrics creating a certain emotion within the listener, is the subject of how lyrics have changed over time. Lyrics have long been used in songs that have had to do with love and so the lyrics helped to elicit feelings of closeness and foster relationships, but more recently in modern times, it has been shown that lyrics in music are beginning to create more antisocial behavior in its listeners. An interesting fact regarding music and emotions is how music has been used to help with mood regulation. In fact, as mentioned in the article already, music is used as a form of therapy, but an intriguing fact regarding music and emotions, is that music therapists often use music to help in the treatment of many mood and personality disorders, often having their patients analyze the lyrics in the music as one of the "most common interventions".

What Evokes Fear in Music

edit

The intricate elements of music that evoke fear shed light on the profound interplay between sound and human emotion. Analysis reveals that dissonant chords, unexpected melodies, and syncopated rhythms heighten tension, while dark lyrical themes and cultural associations further amplify feelings of fear.[82]Through a strategic blend of harmonic tension, melodic progression, and rhythmic elements, music becomes a potent conduit for stirring fear in listeners. These insights underscore the multifaceted nature of musical composition and its ability to evoke intense emotional responses.[83]

Recent the research findings from Aubé et al. (corresponding author: William Aubé) shed light on the physiological responses to fear-inducing music.[84]Their study reveals that fear is indeed a potent emotional stimulus in music, eliciting activation in brain regions associated with emotional processing. Specifically, exposure to brief musical excerpts conveying fear led to significant activity in regions such as the amygdala and anterior insula, known for their involvement in emotion processing and threat detection. Notably, there was heightened response in the anterior superior temporal gyrus (STG), a region sensitive to musical stimuli, when exposed to fearful and joyful music compared to neutral or sad music. This suggests that the intensity of emotion, rather than its specific valence, influences neural responses in this area. Overall, these combined insights underscore the diverse mechanisms through which music can evoke fear and engage similar neural pathways as other forms of emotional expression, such as facial expressions and vocalizations.[84]

Brainwashed effect from music

edit

As highlighted by Ignatowicz (2023), is profound and multifaceted. Music serves as a powerful medium for emotional engagement and spiritual connection within religious settings.[85]The melodies and lyrics chosen for mass can evoke deep emotions and reinforce religious teachings, fostering a sense of unity among worshippers. Additionally, music plays a crucial role in preserving cultural traditions and guiding personal reflection and spiritual growth. This exploration of music's influence on mass beliefs offers valuable insights into the intersection of spirituality, culture, and human experience."

The Persuasive Power of Music (Sonic Rhetoric)

edit

The persuasive power of music, encapsulated within the realm of sonic rhetoric, is a multifaceted phenomenon that significantly influences collective beliefs and ideologies within society.[85] Music's capacity to evoke profound emotional responses serves as a cornerstone for its persuasive impact, transcending linguistic barriers and resonating deeply with individuals across diverse cultural backgrounds.

Beyond merely conveying messages, music possesses a unique ability to tap into human emotions, fostering empathy and connection among listeners.  Moreover, beyond the emotional realm, music also functions as a vehicle for conveying complex ideas and values through symbolic representation. By drawing upon familiar symbols and themes, songs serve as vessels for cultural transmission, reinforcing collective identity and perpetuating shared beliefs and traditions. This symbolic resonance not only strengthens cultural connections but also shapes societal narratives, contributing to the construction of collective memory and the preservation of cultural heritage. [85] [79]

Furthermore, music acts as a powerful unifying force, capable of bringing communities together around shared experiences and values. Whether through communal singing at cultural events or the rhythmic chants of protestors at demonstrations, musical gatherings have the inherent ability to foster a sense of belonging and cohesion among participants.[86] This communal engagement with music not only enhances social bonds but also amplifies the persuasive impact of sonic rhetoric, as collective beliefs and ideologies are reinforced through shared experiences and interactions.  

In addition to its role in shaping collective beliefs and identities, music also catalyzes societal change and cultural evolution. By challenging existing norms and narratives, music has the potential to spark critical reflection and inspire action, driving forward movements for social justice and cultural transformation. Whether through the subversive lyrics of punk rock or the anthemic choruses of civil rights hymns, music has historically played a central role in mobilizing individuals and communities toward meaningful change.[87]

Overall, the persuasive power of music lies not only in its emotive resonance and symbolic richness but also in its ability to foster solidarity, inspire action, and drive cultural evolution. By harnessing the tools of sonic rhetoric, music communicates complex ideas, reinforces cultural identity, and shapes societal narratives, making it a potent force for driving forward progress and fostering collective understanding in an increasingly interconnected world.

References

edit
  1. ^ Cf. http://plato.stanford.edu/entries/music/[full citation needed]
  2. ^ Davies, S. (2005). "Artistic Expression and the Hard Case of Pure Music". In Kieran, M. (ed.). Contemporary Debates in Aesthetics and the Philosophy of Art. pp. 179–191.
  3. ^ Davies, Stephen (2013) [2011]. Musical Understandings and Other Essays on the Philosophy of Music. Oxford University Press. p. 221. ISBN 9780199608775.
  4. ^ Davies 2006, p. 181.
  5. ^ Davies 2006, p. 182.
  6. ^ Davies 2006[page needed]
  7. ^ Jenefer Robinson, Deeper than Reason: Emotion and its Role in Literature, Music, and Art, Oxford: Oxford University Press, 2005; pp. 310-13
  8. ^ "Piaget's theory of cognitive development", Piaget's Theory, Routledge, pp. 32–57, 2013-01-11, doi:10.4324/9780203715796-5, ISBN 978-0-203-71579-6, retrieved 2024-04-28
  9. ^ a b c d e f g Dowling, W.J. (2002). "The development of music perception and cognition". Foundations of Cognitive Psychology: Core Reading: 481–502.
  10. ^ Susino, M.; Schubert, S. (2017). "Cross-cultural anger communication in music: Towards a stereotype theory of emotion in music". Musicae Scientiae. 21: 60–74. doi:10.1177/1029864916637641. S2CID 148139308.
  11. ^ Thompson, William Forde & Balkwill, Laura-Lee (2010). "Chapter 27: Cross-cultural similarities and differences" (PDF). In Juslin, Patrik & Sloboda, John (eds.). Handbook of Music and Emotion: Theory, Research, Applications. Oxford University Press. pp. 755–788. ISBN 978-0-19-960496-8.
  12. ^ a b c d e f g h i j k l m n o p q r s t u v w Scherer, K. R.; Zentner, M. R. (2001). "Emotional effects of music: production rules". Music and Emotion: Theory and Research: 361–387. doi:10.1093/oso/9780192631886.003.0016. ISBN 978-0-19-263188-6.
  13. ^ a b c d Radford, C. (1989). "Emotions and music: A reply to the cognitivists". The Journal of Aesthetics and Art Criticism. 47 (1): 69–76. doi:10.2307/431994. JSTOR 431994.
  14. ^ a b Susino, M.; Schubert, E. (2020). "Musical emotions in the absence of music: A cross-cultural investigation of emotion communication in music by extra-musical cues". PLOS ONE. 15 (11): e0241196. Bibcode:2020PLoSO..1541196S. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0241196. PMC 7673536. PMID 33206664.
  15. ^ a b c d e f Gabrielle, A.; Stromboli, E. (2001). "The influence of musical structure on emotional expression". Music and Emotion: Theory and Research: 223–243.
  16. ^ "Is Music a Universal Language? | Psychology Today". www.psychologytoday.com.
  17. ^ Egermann, H; Fernando, N; Chuen, L; McAdams, S (2014). "Music induces universal emotion-related psychophysiological responses: comparing Canadian listeners to Congolese Pygmies". Front Psychol. 5: 1341. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01341. PMC 4286616. PMID 25620935.
  18. ^ Argstatter, Heike (July 6, 2016). "Perception of basic emotions in music: Culture-specific or multicultural?". Psychology of Music. 44 (4): 674–690. doi:10.1177/0305735615589214 – via CrossRef.
  19. ^ How Art Works (podcast, 10 min 18 sec)
  20. ^ Dellacherie, D., Roy, M., Hugueville, L., Peretz, I., & Samson, S. (2011). The effect of musical experience on emotional self-reports and psychophysiological responses to dissonance. Psychophysiology, 48(3), 337-349. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.2010.01075.x
  21. ^ Gomez, P.; Danuser, B. (2007). "Relationships between musical structure and psychophysiological measures of emotion". Emotion. 7 (2): 377–387. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.463.5753. doi:10.1037/1528-3542.7.2.377. PMID 17516815.
  22. ^ Ellen Winner (2018). How Art Works: A Psychological Exploration. Oxford University Press. ISBN 978-0190863357.
  23. ^ How Art Works (podcast, 6 min 15 sec)
  24. ^ HANSCOMBE, ELISABETH, "Rock-a-Bye Baby", Middle Grounds, Demeter Press, pp. 177–190, doi:10.2307/j.ctv1fxm36.18, retrieved 2024-04-29
  25. ^ Barrett, F. S.; Janata, P. (2016). "Neural responses to nostalgia-evoking music modeled by elements of dynamic musical structure and individual differences in affective traits". Neuropsychologia. 91: 234–246. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2016.08.012. PMID 27526666. S2CID 32435787.
  26. ^ Rich, Jeremy (2015-09-30), "Mercury, Freddie", African American Studies Center, Oxford University Press, doi:10.1093/acref/9780195301731.013.50562, ISBN 978-0-19-530173-1, retrieved 2024-04-28
  27. ^ Friedman, Ronald S. (2018-09-01). "Final Ritardandi and the Expression of Musical Emotion". Music Perception. 36 (1): 53–59. doi:10.1525/mp.2018.36.1.53. ISSN 0730-7829.
  28. ^ Friedman, Ronald S. (2018-09-01). "Final Ritardandi and the Expression of Musical Emotion". Music Perception. 36 (1): 53–59. doi:10.1525/mp.2018.36.1.53. ISSN 0730-7829.
  29. ^ "Does Sad Music Make Humans Happy?". The Science Explorer. Retrieved 2024-04-28.
  30. ^ "Does Sad Music Make Humans Happy?". The Science Explorer. Retrieved 2024-04-28.
  31. ^ van Sprang, Irene; Haeyen, Suzanne (2024-03-25). "Music therapy for therapeutic development in personality disorders: A qualitative case study". Journal of Clinical Psychology. 80 (7): 1596–1606. doi:10.1002/jclp.23682. ISSN 0021-9762. PMID 38523589.
  32. ^ a b c d e f g h i Hunter, P. G.; Schellenburg, E. G.; Schimmack, U. (2010). "Feelings and perceptions of happiness and sadness induced by music: Similarities, differences, and mixed emotions". Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts. 4: 47–56. doi:10.1037/a0016873. S2CID 49568657.
  33. ^ a b c Larsen, J. T.; Stastny, B. J. (2011). "It's a bittersweet symphony: Simultaneously mixed emotional responses to music with conflicting cues". Emotion. 11 (6): 1469–1473. doi:10.1037/a0024081. PMID 21707144.
  34. ^ a b c d e f g h Sloboda, J. A.; Juslin, P. N. (2001). "Psychological perspectives on music and emotion". Music and Emotion: Theory and Research: 79–96.
  35. ^ a b c Nawrot, E. S. (1 January 2003). "The Perception of Emotional Expression in Music: Evidence from Infants, Children and Adults". Psychology of Music. 31 (1): 75–92. doi:10.1177/0305735603031001325. S2CID 144089631.
  36. ^ a b c Mote, Jasmine (1 January 2011). "The effects of tempo and familiarity on children's affective interpretation of music". Emotion. 11 (3): 618–622. doi:10.1037/a0022573. PMID 21668112.
  37. ^ a b c TERWOGT, MARK MEERUM; VAN GRINSVEN, FLORA (1 December 1988). "Recognition of Emotions in Music by Children and Adults". Perceptual and Motor Skills. 67 (3): 697–698. doi:10.2466/pms.1988.67.3.697. PMID 3226819. S2CID 36329792.
  38. ^ a b c Trehub, Sandra E.; Nakata, Takayuki (2001). "Emotion and music in infancy". Musicae Scientiae. SPEC ISSUE, 2001–2002: 37–61. doi:10.1177/10298649020050S103. S2CID 147382224.
  39. ^ Vist, Torill (16 November 2011). "Music Experience in Early Childhood: Potential for Emotion Knowledge?". International Journal of Early Childhood. 43 (3): 277–290. doi:10.1007/s13158-011-0045-7. S2CID 144761804.
  40. ^ "Beyond Twinkle, Twinkle: Using Music with Infants and Toddlers". ZERO TO THREE. Retrieved 2024-04-28.
  41. ^ a b c ROBAZZA, CLAUDIO; MACALUSO, CRISTINA; D'URSO, VALENTINA (1 October 1994). "Emotional Reactions to Music by Gender, Age, and Expertise". Perceptual and Motor Skills. 79 (2): 939–944. doi:10.2466/pms.1994.79.2.939. PMID 7870518. S2CID 22959117.
  42. ^ "Beyond Twinkle, Twinkle: Using Music with Infants and Toddlers". ZERO TO THREE. Retrieved 2024-04-28.
  43. ^ a b Ladinig, Olivia; Schellenberg, E. Glenn (1 January 2012). "Liking unfamiliar music: Effects of felt emotion and individual differences". Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts. 6 (2): 146–154. doi:10.1037/a0024671.
  44. ^ a b Schellenberg, E. Glenn; Mankarious, Monika (1 January 2012). "Music training and emotion comprehension in childhood". Emotion. 12 (5): 887–891. doi:10.1037/a0027971. PMID 22642351.
  45. ^ Vuoskoski, J. K.; Eerola, T. (13 July 2011). "Measuring music-induced emotion: A comparison of emotion models, personality biases, and intensity of experiences". Musicae Scientiae. 15 (2): 159–173. doi:10.1177/1029864911403367. S2CID 144079608.
  46. ^ a b Kratus, J. (1 January 1993). "A Developmental Study of Children's Interpretation of Emotion in Music". Psychology of Music. 21 (1): 3–19. doi:10.1177/030573569302100101. S2CID 145078460.
  47. ^ Peel, Ian (2001). Prodigy, the. Oxford Music Online. Oxford University Press. doi:10.1093/gmo/9781561592630.article.47241.
  48. ^ Council on Communications Media (19 October 2009). "Impact of Music, Music Lyrics, and Music Videos on Children and Youth". Pediatrics. 124 (5): 1488–1494. doi:10.1542/peds.2009-2145. PMID 19841124.
  49. ^ a b c Garrido, S.; E. Shubert (2011). "Individual differences in the enjoyment of negative emotion in music: a literature review and experiment". Music Perception. 28 (3): 279–295. doi:10.1525/MP.2011.28.3.279.
  50. ^ Van den TOl, A.J.M.; Edwards, J. (2013). "Exploring a rationale for choosing to listen to sad music when feeling sad". Psychology of Music.
  51. ^ "Does Sad Music Make Humans Happy?". The Science Explorer. Retrieved 2024-04-28.
  52. ^ a b c d e Ali, S. O.; Peynircioglu, Z. F. (2010). "Intensity of emotions conveyed and elicited by familiar and unfamiliar music". Music Perception. 27 (3): 177–182. doi:10.1525/MP.2010.27.3.177. JSTOR 10.1525/mp.2010.27.3.177.
  53. ^ Van den Tol, A. J. M., & Ritchie, T. D.(book-chapter in press). Emotion memory and music: A critical review and recommendations for future research.Music, Memory and Autobiography. (Eds: Professor Strollo Maria Rosaria and Dr. Romano Alessandra).
  54. ^ Van; den Tol, A. J. M.; Edwards, J. (2013). "Exploring a rationale for choosing to listen to sad music when feeling sad". Psychology of Music. 41 (4): 440–465. doi:10.1177/0305735611430433. S2CID 145108524.
  55. ^ Van; den Tol, A. J. M.; Edwards, J. (2014). "Listening to sad music in adverse situations: Music selection strategies, self-regulatory goals, listening effect, and mood-enhancement" (PDF). Psychology of Music. 43 (4): 473–494. doi:10.1177/0305735613517410. S2CID 145208440.
  56. ^ Patrik Juslin & Daniel V ̈astfj ̈all, 'Emotional responses to music: The need to consider underlying mechanisms, Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 31, 2008; 559-621.
  57. ^ Juslin, Liljeström, Västfjäll, & Lundqvist. (2010). How does music evoke emotions? Exploring the underlying mechanisms. In P.N. Juslin & J. Sloboda (Eds.), Handbook of Music and Emotion: Theory, Research, and Applications (pp. 605-642). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  58. ^ Juslin, P. (2011). Music and Emotion: seven questions, seven answers. In I. Deliège, J. Davidson, J. A. Sloboda (Eds.), Music and the Mind: Essays in Honour of John Sloboda (pp.113-138). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  59. ^ Davies, J. B. (1978). The psychology of music. Hutchinson.
  60. ^ Susino, Marco; Thompson, William Forde; Schubert, Emery; Broughton, Mary (2024-04-07). "Emotional Responses to Music: The Essential Inclusion of Emotion Adaptability and Situational Context". Empirical Studies of the Arts. doi:10.1177/02762374241237683. ISSN 0276-2374.
  61. ^ Maher, P; Van Tilburg, W. A. P.; Van; den Tol, A. J. M. (2013). "Meaning in music: Deviations from expectation in music polarize in-group and out-group attitudes". European Journal of Social Psychology. 34 (2): 155–172. doi:10.1002/ejsp.196.
  62. ^ Juslin, Patrick (2019). What Comes Next? Musical Expectancy', Musical Emotions Explained: Unlocking the Secrets of Musical Affect (online ed.). Oxford Academic. pp. Pages 343–C24. ISBN 9780198753421.
  63. ^ Juslin, P. N. (2013). "From everyday emotions to aesthetic emotions: towards a unified theory of musical emotions". Physics of Life Reviews. 10 (3): 235–266. Bibcode:2013PhLRv..10..235J. doi:10.1016/j.plrev.2013.05.008. PMID 23769678.
  64. ^ Koelsch, Stefan (March 2014). "Brain correlates of music-evoked emotions". Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 15 (3): 170–180. doi:10.1038/nrn3666. ISSN 1471-003X. PMID 24552785.
  65. ^ a b c Vieillard, S.; Peretz, I.; Gosselin, N.; Khalfa, S. (2008). "Happy, sad, scary, and peaceful musical excerpts for research on emotions". Cognition and Emotion. 22 (4): 720–752. doi:10.1080/02699930701503567. S2CID 15310907.
  66. ^ Gabrielsson, A. (2001). "Emotion in strong experiences with music". Music and Emotion: Theory and Research: 431–449. doi:10.1093/oso/9780192631886.003.0019. ISBN 978-0-19-263188-6.
  67. ^ "How Music Can Make Us Feel Good". NAMI California. Retrieved 2024-04-30.
  68. ^ a b c d e Kallenin, K; Ravaja, N. (2006). "Emotion perceived and emotion felt: Same and different". Musicae Scientiae. 10 (2): 191–213. doi:10.1177/102986490601000203. S2CID 143503605.
  69. ^ Schubert, Emery (2007). "Locus of emotion: The effect of task order and age on emotion perceived and emotion felt in response to music". Journal of Music Therapy. 44 (4): 344–368. doi:10.1093/jmt/44.4.344. PMID 17997625.
  70. ^ Zentner, Marcel; Grandjean, Didier; Scherer, Klaus R. (1 January 2008). "Emotions evoked by the sound of music: Characterization, classification, and measurement". Emotion. 8 (4): 494–521. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.459.9866. doi:10.1037/1528-3542.8.4.494. PMID 18729581.
  71. ^ Keen MSocSc, Alexander W. (2005). "Using Music as a Therapy Tool to Motivate Troubled Adolescents". Social Work in Health Care. 39 (3–4): 361–373. doi:10.1300/J010v39n03_09. PMID 15774401. S2CID 25035875.
  72. ^ Baker, Felicity A.; Gleadhill, Libby M.; Dingle, Genevieve A. (2007). "Music therapy and emotional exploration: Exposing substance abuse clients to the experiences of non-drug-induced emotions". The Arts in Psychotherapy. 34 (4): 321–330. doi:10.1016/j.aip.2007.04.005.
  73. ^ Robb, Sheri L. (2000). "The effect of therapeutic music interventions on the behavior of hospitalized children in isolation: Developing a contextual support model of music therapy". Journal of Music Therapy. 37 (2): 118–146. doi:10.1093/jmt/37.2.118. PMID 10932125.
  74. ^ Heaton, Pam (2009). "Music - shelter for the frazzled mind?"". The Psychologist. 22 (12): 1018–1020.
  75. ^ blythehinderliter (2019-03-27). "How Music Therapy Helps Children & Teens Cope with Mental Health Challenges". Camber Children's Mental Health. Retrieved 2024-05-01.
  76. ^ Ignatowicz, Hubert (2023). "THE ROLE OF MUSIC IN CHURCH COMMUNITIES: A CASE OF A POLISH CHURCH COMMUNITY IN LONDON". The Role of Music in Church Community: 40. doi:10.13140/RG.2.2.35631.51363.
  77. ^ Iain McGilchrist. The Master And His Emissary: The Divided Brain And The Making Of The Western World.
  78. ^ Wu, Keyan (2022). "How Lyrics Inflfluence the Effect of Music on Emotions Regarding Differences in Musical Training".
  79. ^ a b Minson, Christopher T. (2009). "Microvascular Measures". Medicine & Science in Sports & Exercise. 41 (5): 71. doi:10.1249/01.mss.0000354369.06827.a1. ISSN 0195-9131.
  80. ^ Nathan, Leigh Jones (2018). "The impact of music and lyrics on emotion" (PDF).
  81. ^ Xu, Liang; Sun, Zaoyi; Wen, Xin; Huang, Zhengxi; Chao, Chi-ju; Xu, Liuchang (2021-11-15). "Using machine learning analysis to interpret the relationship between music emotion and lyric features". PeerJ Computer Science. 7: e785. doi:10.7717/peerj-cs.785. ISSN 2376-5992. PMC 8627224. PMID 34901433.
  82. ^ Sharma, Suraj (2022-08-04). The Transformative Effects of Music In Individual and Collective Sensemaking (Report). Open Science Framework. doi:10.31219/osf.io/s5mb2.
  83. ^ Schmuckler, Mark A.; Boltz, Marilyn G. (1994). "Harmonic and rhythmic influences on musical expectancy". Perception & Psychophysics. 56 (3): 313–325. doi:10.3758/BF03209765. ISSN 0031-5117. PMID 7971131.
  84. ^ a b Aubé, William; Angulo-Perkins, Arafat; Peretz, Isabelle; Concha, Luis; Armony, Jorge L. (2015-03-01). "Fear across the senses: brain responses to music, vocalizations and facial expressions". Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience. 10 (3): 399–407. doi:10.1093/scan/nsu067. ISSN 1749-5024. PMC 4350486. PMID 24795437.
  85. ^ a b c Ignatowicz, Hubert (2023). "THE ROLE OF MUSIC IN CHURCH COMMUNITIES: A CASE OF A POLISH CHURCH COMMUNITY IN LONDON". The Role of Music in Church Community: 40. doi:10.13140/RG.2.2.35631.51363.
  86. ^ "Resonating Rhythms: The Power of Music". MDLBEAST. Retrieved 2024-03-26.
  87. ^ Liu, Jiayi; Chen, Yan; Huang, Shu (2024). Khan, I.A.; Cüneyt Birkök, M.; Rak, J.; Haratyan, F. (eds.). "Exploring the Sociocultural Influence of Music Traditions Across Different Cultures". SHS Web of Conferences. 187: 02008. doi:10.1051/shsconf/202418702008. ISSN 2261-2424.

Further reading

edit

This article incorporates material from the Citizendium article "Music and emotion", which is licensed under the Creative Commons Attribution-ShareAlike 3.0 Unported License but not under the GFDL.